Laikun Ma, Wei Liang. 2021: Egg rejection and egg recognition mechanisms in Oriental Reed Warblers. Avian Research, 12(1): 47. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-021-00283-4
Citation: Laikun Ma, Wei Liang. 2021: Egg rejection and egg recognition mechanisms in Oriental Reed Warblers. Avian Research, 12(1): 47. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-021-00283-4

Egg rejection and egg recognition mechanisms in Oriental Reed Warblers

Funds: 

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31970427

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 32101242

the Open Foundation of Hebei Key Laboratory of Wetland Ecology and Conservation hklk201903

the Natural Science Foundation of Hebei Province of China C2020101002

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Wei Liang, liangwei@hainnu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 18 Jun 2021
  • Accepted Date: 06 Sep 2021
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 20 Sep 2021
  • Background 

    Nest parasitism by cuckoos (Cuculus spp.) results in enormous reproductive failure and forces hosts to evolve antiparasitic strategies, i.e., recognition of own eggs and rejection of cuckoo eggs. There are often sexual conflicts between male and female individuals in the expression of antiparasitic behavior due to the differences in reproductive inputs and division of labor.

    Methods 

    By adding a foreign egg made of blue soft clay to the host nest during early incubation period in the field, and by removing several host eggs and adding experimental eggs to control the proportion of two egg types in the nest, we examined egg rejection ability, egg recognition mechanism and sexual difference in egg rejection of the Oriental Reed Warbler (Acrocephalus orientalis), one of the major hosts of Common Cuckoos (Cuculus canorus).

    Results 

    Our results indicated that Oriental Reed Warblers can recognize and reject nearly 100% (73/75) of the non-mimetic eggs made of blue soft clay, and they could reject foreign eggs with 100% accuracy, regardless of the ratio of experimental eggs and its own eggs in the nest. Furthermore, all cases of egg rejections recorded by videos were only carried out by females.

    Conclusions 

    Oriental Reed Warblers have a high egg recognition ability and show a true recognition mechanism. Only female warblers perform egg rejection, suggesting that the sex for host egg incubation seems to play an important role in the evolution of egg recognition mechanisms.

  • Deserts have received less attention than other climates for seed dispersal for at least two reasons easily found in literature (e.g. Ellner and Shmida 1981): (1) since desert plants tend to delay germination until the occurrence of rare precipitation events, their survival has more often been understood as dependent on dormancy than on dispersal; (2) assumed lack of devices for long-range dispersal, or presence of traits that hinder dispersal, in their seeds has been explained with the rarity of microsites enhancing seedling establishment. However, these reasons lose value if one considers that: (1) dormancy and dispersal are not necessarily in conflict with each other, as plants of extreme desert environments produce long-living seed banks with deep dormancy as well as having their seeds dispersed by wind in active sand dunes over considerable distances (e.g. Calligonum mongolicum in Fan et al. 2018); (2) animals might search for useful microsites to save seeds for later consumption. Such sites might also be suitable for seedling establishment and the animal might fail to recover all the cached seeds, thus favoring the dispersal of the plant. The latter mode of seed dispersal, called synzoochory, is a well-known occurrence in temperate and tropical climates, but not as well in deserts. In their recent global review on synzoochory based on 2223 case studies, Gómez et al. (2019) included the deserts in a broader Arid category, for which they remarked on the prevalence of rodents and listed (see Appendix S1 in Gómez et al. 2019) only six species of birds among the dispersers. Five of those birds belong in the crow family: Aphelocoma californica, Cyanocitta stelleri, Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus and Nucifraga columbiana from northern America and Garrulus glandarius from Europe. These corvids mainly disperse pines and oaks and are clearly not true desert birds. The same goes for the one non-corvid bird, the American Acorn Woodpecker (Melanerpes formicivorus). In the same paper, a lack of information on synzoochory from large parts of the world was admitted and no study case from Central Asia was included in the Arid category. In fact, considerable attention has been paid to animal-plant interaction in arid northwestern China, but only ants were reported as seed-dispersing animals (Liu et al. 2014), while Rhombomys opimus, a widespread Central Asian rodent and conceivably a seed-disperser, suggested different relationships with desert plants (Xu et al. 2015).

    Desert birds have mainly been studied as frugivores, classical endozoochorous animals which disperse seeds through gut passage of fleshy fruits (van Rheedevan Oudtshorn and van Rooyen 1999; Bronstein et al. 2007). The occurrence of frugivores in arid environments is limited by the rarity of fleshy-fruited plants in the lack of water, and consequently, birds have not been considered as important seed dispersers in deserts. Some corvids are well-known avian counterparts of the scatter-hoarding rodents in the temperate zone of the northern hemisphere. Pesendorfer et al. (2016) highlighted their unique aspects as seed dispersers: compared to rodents, corvids are more selective on the quality of the seeds to be cached, disperse them over longer distances, and are not as hesitant to cross unfavorable spaces in fragmented habitats, thus reducing post-dispersal predation (pilferage) from the rodents. De Kort and Clayton (2006) lists 43 corvid species from moderate to specialized food cachers, plus only two non-cachers: the specialized cachers are all inhabitants of well wooded areas, while the moderate cachers use more diverse habitats. The inclusion of a desert species, a ground-jay, in the moderate cachers is based on Bardin (2006), though the behavioural description by Bardin would rather suggest the specialized cacher, as reported in the following content for evidence for ground-jays as seed dispersers in deserts. Unfortunately, no equally informative description exists either for this or other ground-jay species as food cachers.

    There is sparse, though growing, information on the general habits of these unusual corvids of Central Asian deserts, more prone to move around on foot than in flight, hence the generic name Podoces, in no larger associations than family groups. Madge (2009) gives the most comprehensive overview of the four species. Among their shared traits, especially pertinent to the present topic is that they live in arid environments with strong seasonality and consequently they change from mainly animal to mainly vegetal diets in the cold season. No quantitative study exists of their relationship with seed dispersal. The Turkestan Ground-jay (Podoces panderi) is probably the best studied species. Kovshar (2015) reports Bardin's (2006) description of birds collecting and burying Haloxylon seeds in the Karakum Desert, in October. Several points in that detailed description suggest the specialized seed-disperser. Weighing "about 5mg each" (see also Liu et al.2014), Haloxylon seeds would be the smallest seeds dispersed by seed-storing corvids (compare with mass values of Table 7.2 in Tomback 2016). The "hyoid bag" where the Turkestan Ground-jays placed "several hundred seeds" reminds of the sublingual pouch of the Nucifraga nutcrackers, an adaptation of some of the best-known seed-dispersers to carry seeds efficiently over potentially extensive distances (Bock et al. 1973). The caching technique, through which the stocks were hidden by covering the holes with well-leveled sand, or pieces of other surrounding material, reveals the adaptation to avoiding that caches are stolen by other animals, including conspecifics. This is a well-known corvid behaviour (see e.g. description and fine photographs in Kurek et al. 2018 concerning Garrulus glandarius). The moderate burial depth, 2.5–4cm, was close to the optimum for germination and seedling emergence in a plant of similar habitat (Fan et al.2018). The Russian common name for the Turkestan Ground-jay, saksaul'naya soika (Saxaul Jay), mirrors its habitat, a sandy desert with saxaul shrub, Haloxylon aphyllum or Haloxylon persicum, dominant. As neither Haloxylon species produces seeds every year (Rustamov 1994), it may be interesting to know whether the birds move among saxaul grows that fruit in different years, or they can alternatively rely on other plants for seed provision. Kovshar (2015) reported the presence of the seeds of other desert plants in stomach contents, although with no suggestion that they were from caches. Zhatkanbayev (2015) observed Turkestan Ground-jays in the snow while plucking the remaining fruits from emergent clumps not only of Haloxylon but also of Stipagrostis and other "low-growing desert plants", in addition to apparently searching for previous caches.

    The Iranian Ground-jay (Podoces pleskei) is a sister species of the Turkestan Ground-jay and lives in less arid areas, where it nests on a variety of shrubs and bushes (e.g. Nazarizadeh et al. 2015). Although its biology is increasingly studied, little is known about its possible relationship with seed dispersal. Hamedanian (1997) stated that "this species holds some extra food in its throat sac before burying it in holes in the ground", and claimed that, "as hiding food is a habit of members of the family and Pleske's Ground Jay feeds almost entirely on plant seeds in the autumn, so the bird can play an important role in planting different scrubs and borage plants of its desert habitat". Radnezhad et al. (2011) found seeds of Zygophyllum eurypterum in the fecal sacs of nestlings, arguably no later than in May, the later time of fledging found in the study areas. As the seeds of this shrub apparently begin to mature at the end of June in Iran (Ghasemi et al. 2017), it seems likely that the ingested seeds were from caches of the previous year. The Mongolian Ground-jay (Podoces hendersoni) is adapted to the coldest deserts of Central Asia and prefers gravel surface, even with very scarce vegetation. Kurochkin and Mikhailov (1994) observed birds burying almond nuts in cracks of the soil, and Ilyashenko et al. (2017) reported chicks being fed in April with Amygdalus mongolica kernels taken by the parents from under stones. Considering that this plant fruits in August–September (http://www.efloras.org), this observation seems to be the best available evidence for any ground-jay species that autumn caches are used as spring food. It may be worth noting for future research that Mongolian Ground-jays were found widespread in the Junggar Basin, northwestern China (Londei 2000), where wild Amygdalus almonds are absent (http://www.efloras.org) and thus this ground-jay might also rely on caches from other plants. The Xinjiang Ground-jay (Podoces biddulphi) has its distribution surrounded by, but not mixed with, the distribution of the Mongolian Ground-jay, of which it is the closest relative. It is endemic to the Taklimakan Desert and able to live even in the harsh interior of this sandy desert, providing that groundwater is accessible to vegetation in low areas between the dunes (Ma and Kwok 2004; Ma 2011). The artificial irrigated vegetal belt that protects the Taklimakan Desert Highway from encroaching sand has been found to positively affect the occurrence of Xinjiang Ground-jays along the road, a phenomenon ascribed to these plants providing cover, drinking water, food, and nesting sites (Londei 2011; Xu et al. 2013). These birds were found to have seeds in their diet and were also observed, in the end of summer, collecting food left by humans along the road and quickly burying it in the sand, rather than eating it (Ma 2004). It may be worth investigating whether the vegetal belt itself benefits from the presence of the birds, in terms of better survival of buried seeds.

    Future study of the ground jays will likely take advantage of comparison with the vast literature on those corvids that are well-known seed-dispersers in temperate climates. The references in de Kort and Clayton (2006) are a useful source of anatomical and eco-ethological studies, as well as experimental tests, of food-caching behaviour in corvids. Ground-jays suggest a seemingly unexplored connection of this behaviour with the omnivorous strategy of all corvids: Turkestan Ground-jays were observed, in winter, finding the egg capsules of the Desert Locust (Caliptamus barbarus) under snow-covered soil (Zhatkanbayev and Zhatkanbayeva 2017) and it seems therefore possible that their ability in observational learning, necessary to recover their own caches and also useful to steal the others', makes them also especially fit for detecting hidden animal prey. If so, the ground-jays might be of further advantage to desert vegetation, by performing pest control during critical stages of the life of the pest. The desert plants mentioned in the present paper as candidates for having their seeds dispersed by ground-jays seem all important for environmental maintenance and restoration. Calligonum and Haloxylon genera, which were both found as seeds in the stomachs of Turkestan Ground-jays (Rustamov 1954; Kovshar 2015), were found in China to be important tools in desert afforestation (Zhang et al. 2016). Zygophyllum eurypterum has been studied among important rangeland plants with dormant seeds that rarely germinate in "usual natural conditions" (Sharififar et al. 2015)—it might be worth investigating whether ground-jays provide the seeds with more favorable natural conditions than those usually considered.Amygdalus mongolica, a shrub of gravel semidesert that survives in refugia unaffected by overgrazing, seems to be a promising candidate for rangeland restoration (Chu et al. 2015). The ground-jays may be more common than expected for so scarcely studied birds. Although concern for degradation of their habitats appears in literature (see Burnside et al. 2020 for a recent account), one should consider that, as usual in corvids, ground-jays have benefited from some man-modified environment, see the Taklimakan Desert Highway. Such adaptability might make their ecosystem services especially useful in transitional situations towards environmental restoration. Compared to winds and seasonal floods, the usual abiotic agents of seed dispersal in deserts, the ground-jays would choose where leave the seeds for keeping them viable in their own interest of future consumers, and the plants might have obtained better germination in such environments from a co-evolution with the ground-jays resulting in mutualistic symbiosis. Given their geographical distribution and the recurrent mention in literature of "other plants" in addition to those specified in their diets, the ground-jays might serve as seed dispersers for a variety of plants throughout the most extended continuum of arid and semi-arid land on Earth.

    Ruben H. Heleno and Valentin Yu. Ilyashenko suggested important papers for full appraisal of the topic.

    Not applicable.

    Not applicable.

    Not applicable.

    Not applicable.

    The author declare that he has no competing interests.

  • Antonov A, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Egg rejection in marsh warblers (Acrocephalus palustris) heavily parasitized by common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus). Auk. 2006;123: 419-30.
    Davies NB. Cuckoos, cowbirds and other cheats. London: T & AD Poyser; 2000.
    Davies NB, deL Brooke M. Cuckoos versus reed warblers: adaptations and counteradaptations. Anim Behav. 1988;36: 262-84.
    Davies NB, deL Brooke M. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. I. Host egg discrimination. J Anim Ecol. 1989a;58: 207-24.
    Davies NB, deL Brooke M. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. Ⅱ. Host egg markings, chick discrimination and general discussion. J Anim Ecol. 1989b;58: 225-36.
    deL Brooke M, Davies NB, Noble DG. Rapid decline of host defences in response to reduced cuckoo parasitism: behavioural flexibility of reed warblers in a changing world. Proc R Soc Lond B-Biol Sci. 1998;265: 1277-82.
    Geltsch N, Bán M, Hauber ME, Moskát C. When should common cuckoos Cuculus canorus lay their eggs in host nests? Bird Study. 2016;63: 1-6.
    Hauber ME, Sherman PW. Self-referent phenotype matching: theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24: 609-16.
    Honza M, Procházka P, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Čapek M, et al. Are blackcaps current winners in the evolutionary struggle against the common cuckoo? J Ethol. 2004;22: 175-80.
    Lahti DC. Evolution of bird eggs in the absence of cuckoo parasitism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102: 18057-62.
    Lahti DC. Persistence of egg recognition in the absence of cuckoo brood parasitism: pattern and mechanism. Evolution. 2006;60: 157-68.
    Li D, Wei H, Zhang Z, Liang W, Stokke BG. Oriental reed warbler (Acrocephalus orientalis) nest defence behaviour towards brood parasites and nest predators. Behaviour. 2015;152: 1601-21.
    Li D, Zhang Z, Grim T, Liang W, Stokke BG. Explaining variation in brood parasitism rates between potential host species with similar habitat requirements. Evol Ecol. 2016;30: 905-23.
    Li D, Li X, Zhang Y, Guan S, Ruan Y. Contrasting effects of egg size and appearance on egg recognition and rejection response by Oriental reed warblers. Ecol Evol. 2020;10: 11523-34.
    Liang W, Yang C, Antonov A, Fossøy F, Stokke BG, Moksnes A. Sex roles in egg recognition and egg polymorphism in avian brood parasitism. Behav Ecol. 2012;23: 397-402.
    Liang W, Møller AP, Stokke BG, Yang C, Kovařík P, Wang H, et al. Geographic variation in egg ejection rate by great tits across 2 continents. Behav Ecol. 2016;27: 1405-12.
    Lindholm AK, Thomas RJ. Differences between populations of reed warblers in defences against brood parasitism. Behaviour. 2000;137: 25-42.
    Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. Rejection of cuckoo eggs in relation to host age: a possible evolutionary equilibrium. Behav Ecol. 1992;3: 128-32.
    Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. Constraints on egg discrimination and cuckoo-host co-evolution. Anim Behav. 1995;49: 1185-209.
    Lyon B. Mechanism of egg recognition in defenses against conspecific brood parasitism: American coots (Fulica americana) know their own eggs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2007;61: 455-63.
    Ma L, Yang C, Liang W. Hawk mimicry does not reduce attacks of cuckoos by highly aggressive hosts. Avian Res. 2018a;9: 35.
    Ma L, Yang C, Liu J, Zhang J, Liang W, Møller AP. Costs of breeding far away from neighbors: Isolated host nests are more vulnerable to cuckoo parasitism. Behav Proc. 2018b;157: 327-32.
    Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Braa AT. Rejection behavior by common cuckoo hosts towards artificial brood parasite eggs. Auk. 1991a;108: 348-54.
    Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Lampe HM. Behavioural responses of potential hosts towards artificial cuckoo eggs and dummies. Behaviour. 1991b;116: 64-89.
    Montgomerie RD, Weatherhead PJ. Risks and rewards of nest defence by parent birds. Q Rev Biol. 1988;63: 167-87.
    Moskát C, Hauber ME. Conflict between egg recognition and egg rejection decisions in common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) hosts. Anim Cogn. 2007;10: 377-86.
    Moskát C, Szentpéteri J, Barta Z. Adaptations by great reed warblers to brood parasitism: A comparison of populations in sympatry and allopatry with the common cuckoo. Behaviour. 2002;139: 1313-29.
    Moskát C, Hansson B, Barabás L, Bártol I, Karcza Z. Common cuckoo Cuculus canorus parasitism, antiparasite defence and gene flow in closely located populations of great reed warblers Acrocephalus arundinaceus. J Avian Biol. 2008;39: 663-71.
    Moskát C, Bán M, Székely T, Komdeur J, Lucassen RW, van Boheemen LA, et al. Discordancy or template-based recognition? Dissecting the cognitive basis of the rejection of foreign eggs in hosts of avian brood parasites. J Exp Biol. 2010;213: 1976-83.
    Moskát C, Takasu F, Munoz AR, Nakamura H, Bán M, Barta Z. Cuckoo parasitism on two closely-related Acrocephalus warblers in distant areas: A case of parallel coevolution. Chin Birds. 2012;3: 320-9.
    Moskát C, Zölei A, Bán M, Elek Z, Tong L, Geltsch N, et al. How to spot a stranger's egg? A mimicry-specific discordancy effect in the recognition of parasitic eggs. Ethology. 2014;120: 616-26.
    Payne RB. The ecology of brood parasitism in birds. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 1977;8: 1-28.
    Peer BD, Rothstein SI, Delaney KS, Fleischer RC. Defence behaviour against brood parasitism is deeply rooted in mainland and island scrub-jays. Anim Behav. 2007;73: 55-63.
    Peer BD, Kuehn MJ, Rothstein SI, Fleischer RC. Persistence of host defence behaviour in the absence of avian brood parasitism. Biol Lett. 2011;7: 670-3.
    Požgayová M, Procházka P, Honza M. Sex-specific defence behaviour against brood parasitism in a host with female-only incubation. Behav Proc. 2009;81: 34-8.
    Rensch B. Verhalten von SingvöelnbeiÄderung des Geleges. Ornithol Monatsber. 1925;33: 169-73.
    Roncalli G, Ibáñez-Álamo D, Soler M. Size and material of model parasitic eggs affect the rejection response of western Bonelli's warbler Phylloscopus bonelli. Ibis. 2017;159: 113-23.
    Rothstein SI. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: Possible learned and innate factors. Auk. 1974;91: 796-807.
    Rothstein SI. Mechanism of egg recognition: Do birds know their own eggs? Anim Behav. 1975;23: 268-78.
    Soler M. Long-term coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Biol Rev. 2014;89: 688-704.
    Soler M, Fernández-Morante J, Espinosa F, Martín-Vivaldi M. Pecking but accepting the parasitic eggs may not reflect ejection failure: the role of motivation. Ethology. 2012;118: 662-72.
    Soler M, Ruiz-Raya F, Roncalli G, Ibáñez-Álamo JD. Relationships between egg-recognition and egg-ejection in a grasp-ejector species. PLoS ONE. 2017;12: e0166283.
    Stokke BG, Rudolfsen G, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Rejection of conspecific eggs in chaffinches: the effect of age and clutch characteristics. Ethology. 2004;110: 459-70.
    Trnka A, Prokop P. Does social mating system influence nest defence behaviour in great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) males? Ethology. 2010;116: 1075-83.
    Trnka A, Požgayová M, Samaš P, Honza M. Repeatability of host female and male aggression towards a brood parasite. Ethology. 2013;119: 907-17.
    Wang L, Yang C, Møller AP, Liang W, Lu X. Multiple mechanisms of egg recognition in a cuckoo host. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2015;69: 1761-7.
    Wang L, Yang C, He G, Liang W, Møller AP. Cuckoos use host egg number to choose host nests for parasitism. Proc R Soc B. 2020;287: 20200343.
    Wang L, He G, Zhang Y, Ma J, Liang W. Cryptic eggs are rejected less frequently by a cuckoo host. Anim Cogn. 2021. .
    Yang C, Li D, Wang L, Liang G, Zhang Z, Liang W. Geographic variation in parasitism rates of two sympatric cuckoo hosts in China. Zool Res. 2014a;35: 67-71.
    Yang C, Liu Y, Zeng L, Liang W. Egg color variation, but not egg rejection behavior, changes in a cuckoo host breeding in the absence of brood parasitism. Ecol Evol. 2014b;4: 2239-46.
    Yang C, Møller AP, Røskaft E, Moksnes A, Liang W, Stokkle BG. Reject the odd egg: egg recognition mechanisms in parrotbills. Behav Ecol. 2014c;25: 1320-4.
    Yang C, Wang L, Liang W, Møller AP. Egg recognition as antiparasitism defence in hosts does not select for laying of matching eggs in parasitic cuckoos. Anim Behav. 2016;122: 177-81.
    Yang C, Wang L, Liang W, Møller AP. How cuckoos find and choose host nests for parasitism. Behav Ecol. 2017;28: 859-65.
    Yang C, Wang L, Liang W, Møller AP. High egg rejection rate in a Chinese population of grey-backed thrush (Turdus hortulorum). Zool Res. 2019;40: 226-30.
    Yang C, Wang L, Cheng S-J, Hsu Y-C, Møller AP, Liang W. Model eggs fail to detect egg recognition in host populations after brood parasitism is relaxed. Front Zool. 2020;17: 14.
    Yang C, Huang J, Liang W, Møller AP. Absence of anti-parasitic defenses in an Asian population of the magpie, a regular host of the great spotted cuckoo in Europe. Curr Zool. 2021;67: 345-7.
    Yi T, Sun YH, Liang W. Egg rejection and egg recognition mechanism of chestnut thrushes (Turdus rubrocanus). Behav Proc. 2020;178: 104158.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(1)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (415) PDF downloads (7) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return