J. H. VAN NIEKERK. 2010: Social organization of a flock of Helmeted Guineafowl (Numida meleagris) at the Krugersdorp Game Reserve, South Africa. Avian Research, 1(1): 22-29. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2009.0004
Citation: J. H. VAN NIEKERK. 2010: Social organization of a flock of Helmeted Guineafowl (Numida meleagris) at the Krugersdorp Game Reserve, South Africa. Avian Research, 1(1): 22-29. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2009.0004

Social organization of a flock of Helmeted Guineafowl (Numida meleagris) at the Krugersdorp Game Reserve, South Africa

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    J. H. VAN NIEKERK, enterprize1@telkomsa.net

  • Received Date: 08 Nov 2009
  • Accepted Date: 14 Jan 2010
  • Available Online: 12 May 2023
  • The social organization of marked Helmeted Guineafowl (Numida meleagris) was studied in the Krugersdorp Game Reserve (Gauteng Province, South Africa) during March 1982 to February 1984. Flock members (7-10 guineafowl) did not associate randomly. Helmeted Guineafowl must live in a sociable flock to survive in the wild. The highest ranking male (male A) formed the pivot of the daily activities of the flock, such as determining foraging direction. High ranking males (males A and B) associated closely to repulse conspecific intruders. Breeding females associated more often with the high ranking males during the breeding season. Although a pecking order was established among males, the frequency of agonistic actions within the flock was very low, which suggests that agonistic interactions are limited to maintain cohesion. Adult females remained between the dominant male and the juveniles to minimize victimization. The second highest ranking male (B) took center stage while the dominant male and his female left temporarily to breed and then the rest of the flock clustered around him to maintain the cohesive nature of the flock. Most adults assisted with the brooding of the chicks of the highest ranking male. Flocking is, among other functions, a predator surveillance strategy that enables the Helmeted Guineafowl to forage under conditions with very limited ground cover and to maximize food finding during winter.

  • Trait polymorphism in natural populations can evolve as a consequence of frequency-dependent selection (Majerus, 1998). This implies that parasites, predators or other selective agents impose variable intensities of selection on the phenotype depending on the frequency in the population. Brood parasites and their hosts provide one such possible case of frequencydependent selection resulting in the evolution of polymorphic eggs in both host and parasite (Kilner, 2006; Yang et al., 2010a). Obligate avian brood parasites lay their eggs in nests belonging to other species of birds, thereby transferring the costs of parental care to their victims. As a consequence, hosts evolve defenses to counter brood parasitism, which in turn selects for corresponding counter-adaptations for better trickery of parasites (Davies and Brooke, 1989). The well-known arms race between parasitic cuckoos and their hosts are regarded as a textbook example of co-evolutionary interactions. Theoretically, the cuckoo-host system, when acting in a frequency-dependent manner, should be able to produce polymorphisms in co-evolved traits in the interacting parties. This hypothetical scenario has been found in the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and one of its hosts, the Ashy-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus), in which both species have evolved matching egg polymorphism manifested in discrete immaculate white, pale blue and blue egg phenotypes within a single population (Fig. 1; Yang et al., 2010a). However, egg mimicry assessment is not always straightforward. Inspection using spectrophotometric methods suggested that the eggs of the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius) were not significantly related to the appearance of its Magpie (Pica pica) host eggs (Soler et al., 2003). In the Red-chested Cuckoo (Cuculus solitarius), cuckoo eggs actually match the eggs of their hosts most closely at wavelengths that cannot be perceived by the human eye (Cherry and Bennett, 2001). Starling et al. (2006) revealed by reflectance spectrophotometry that the color of Pallid Cuckoo (Cuculus pallidus) eggs differed between four host species of Melaphagid Honeyeaters (Lichenostomus penicillatus, L. chrysops, L. melanops, and Melithreptus affinis), and mimicked their hosts' eggs closely in both spectral shape and brightness. The Pallid Cuckoo eggs from the four different hosts' nests matched their respective hosts closely. However, host eggs exhibited a small peak in the ultraviolet that was not mimicked by the cuckoo eggs (Starling et al., 2006). Using digital image analysis and modelling of avian vision, Stoddard and Stevens (2010) recently showed that various features of host egg pattern are mimicked by the eggs of their respective cuckoo host-race. These studies revealed that cuckoos have host-specific egg types that have not been detected by human observation, and emphasize potential inadequacy of human comparisons applied to the coloration of bird eggs, and the importance of techniques such as spectrophotometry to measure color objectively (Starling et al., 2006).

    Figure  1.  Egg polymorphism of Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and Ashy-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus). (a), (b) and (c) refer to blue, pale blue and white clutches of parrotbill, respectively, with a cuckoo egg (larger egg) being present in each clutch (Photos by C. Yang).

    The objective of this study was to quantify egg color by spectrophotometry and assess the extent of egg mimicry of Common Cuckoo to the eggs of its Ashythroated Parrotbill host for blue, pale blue and white clutches, respectively.

    The study was performed in the Kuankuoshui Nature Reserve, Guizhou, south-western China (28°10′N, 107° 10′E) during April–July 2008–2009. The study site is situated in a subtropical moist broadleaf and mixed forest, interspersed with abandoned tea plantations, shrubby areas, and open fields used as cattle pastures (see also Yang et al., 2010a, b).

    Nests were found by systematically searching all typical and potential nest sites and by monitoring the activities of adult hosts throughout the breeding season. We recorded date of the first egg laid, egg color morph, clutch size and occurrence of brood parasitism for each nest. When a nest was found during the incubation period, eggs were floated in water to estimate approximate laying date (Hays and Lecroy, 1971). We used three spectrophotometers for quantification of egg coloration: the USB4000-VIS-NIR, GZ03P and Avantes-2048 to measure the visible (VIS) range (400–700 nm) of blue and white clutches, ultraviolet (UV) range (300–400 nm) of blue and white clutches (Fig. 2) and VIS-UV range (300–700 nm) of pale blue clutches (Fig. 3), respectively (Yang et al., 2009, 2011). Due to equipment limit, we did in such way, which was surely a suboptimal way of doing it. In earlier years, our spectrophotometer can only measure the spectrum range from 400 to 700 nm (VIS). And an additional UV spectrophotometer was used to supplement the UV data. But these data are from quite different machines and cannot be merged together. Finally, the pale blue eggs were measured by the Avantes spectrophotometer which covers the spectrum range from 300–700 nm. However, cuckoo eggs were few and phenotypes we found were very variable in different years.

    Figure  2.  Ultraviolet and visible reflectance spectrum of the egg phenotypes in Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and Ashy-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus). The curves represent the spectrum for one cuckoo egg and average spectra for all host eggs in the observed parasitized nest. B1 and B2 refer to the UV and VIS spectrum of blue clutches; W1 and W2 refer to the UV and VIS spectrum of white clutches.
    Figure  3.  Ultraviolet and visible reflectance spectra of the pale blue egg phenotype in Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and Ashythroated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus). The curves represent the spectrum for one cuckoo egg and average spectra for all host eggs in the observed parasitized nest.

    Both the Ashy-throated Parrotbill (hereafter parrotbill) and the Common Cuckoo (hereafter cuckoo) laid immaculate eggs (Fig. 1), and we obtained six measurements of spectral reflectance for each egg, with two at the blunt end, two at the middle and two at the sharp end of the egg. To represent the egg coloration of the cuckoo, the mean of each egg was summarized from these six measurements. For the parrotbill, egg coloration was represented as the mean of all host eggs in each clutch. Each measurement covered ca. 1 mm2 and was taken at a 45° angle to the egg surface, with the spectrometer and light source connected with a coaxial reflectance probe (Yang et al., 2009, 2010b). We also classified the degree of cuckoo eggs mimicry on a 5-degree scale based on human vision relying on 30 volunteers who scored the degree of mimicry (contrast) from 1 (non-mimetic) to 5 (perfect mimicry) following the approach first developed by Moksnes and Røskaft (1995).

    The experiments comply with the current laws of China in which they were performed. Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University.

    Data analyses were performed in SPSS 13.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois). One-way ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA were used for comparison of normally and non-normally distributed data, respectively. Values were presented as mean ± SD.

    Mimicry score based on human vision showed that the contrasts between cuckoo and parrotbill eggs of the matched-phenotype (blue versus blue, pale blue versus pale blue, and white versus white) differed significantly among the three egg phenotypes (χ2 = 4.41, df = 2, p = 0.015). The mimicry of blue cuckoo eggs to blue host eggs was the highest and significantly higher than that of the white matched pair (blue: 1.03 ± 0.18 vs. white: 1.30 ± 0.47, n = 30 for each category, p = 0.015, post hoc test). The mimicry of pale blue cuckoo egg was intermediate between the blue and the white egg (1.13 ± 0.35, n = 30), with no statistical significant difference from blue (p = 0.273) or white eggs (p = 0.070).

    Egg reflectance spectra revealed that the wave shape, wave peak and wave trough of cuckoo and parrotbill egg spectrum for the blue phenotype were perfectly matching in both visible (VIS) and ultraviolet (UV) ranges (Figs. 12), which indicated that they were very similar in egg color hue and chroma. However, the wave shape of the white cuckoo egg was more variable, with a wave peak in the blue region (Fig. 2), which were lacking in the parrotbill egg. The reflectance spectra for UV between white cuckoo and parrotbill eggs were discrete in 300–340 nm. A similar pattern was found for pale blue cuckoo and parrotbill eggs for which the reflectance curves matched well in wavelengths 340–700 nm.

    Our results show that egg reflectance spectra agree well with the assessment based on human vision that cuckoo eggs mimic those of the parrotbill host. Our previous studies have also indicated that the classification of parrotbill egg morphs based on human vision is consistent with avian visual modelling (Yang et al., 2010a). The sensitivities of UVS-receptor of many birds are concentrated around 340–400 nm with a peak at 370 nm (Chen et al., 1984; Bennett et al., 1994). Recent work by Aidala et al. (2012) also showed that both the Shining Cuckoo (Chalcites [Chrysococcyx] lucidus) and the Long-tailed Cuckoo (Urodynamis [Eudynamis] taitensis) in New Zealand are predicted to possess the short wavelength-sensitive type 1 (SWS1) opsins with maximal sensitivity in the human-visible violet portion of the short-wavelength light spectrum, and not in the UV. Therefore, the UV curves for the three egg phenotypes in cuckoo and its parrotbill host should be regarded as well matching.

    The likelihood of nest predation was not significantly different between nests with white and blue egg in the parrotbill (Yang et al., 2010a). Furthermore, other Paradoxornis species that have no known history of interaction with the cuckoo lay monomorphic eggs in blue color (Jiang et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011). Given that the cuckoo ancestrally had egg colors that were neither white nor blue (Davies, 2000), it was reasonable to conclude that nest predation is not responsible for the evolution of egg polymorphism in the parrotbill, and selection on the cuckoo for countering the evolution of multiple parrotbill egg types was evidenced by hosts generally having evolved good abilities to reject even partly mimetic eggs (Yang et al., 2010a).

    However, we found that mimicry of blue cuckoo eggs is better than that of white cuckoo eggs in their corresponding host clutches, implying that the white morph may potentially be a secondary egg morph that has not yet evolved fine mimetic features.

    In conclusion, we have shown evidence from photospectrometry that different egg color morphs in the Cuckoo have evolved in response to selection against poor mimics imposed by parrotbill hosts. This evidence supports the hypothesis that the white egg morph in the cuckoo-parrotbill system might be a secondary phenotype that has evolved under the strong selection pressure of brood parasitism.

    We are grateful to Anders P. Møller for valuable comments that significantly improved the quality of the manuscript. We thank Eivin Røskaft, Bård G. Stokke and one anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on our manuscript. This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 31071938 and 31272328 to WL, 31101646 and 31260514 to CY), Program for New Century Excellent Talents in University (NCET-10-0111 to WL), and Key Project of Chinese Ministry of Education (No. 212136 to CY). We thank the Forestry Department of Guizhou Province and Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserves for support and permission to carry out this study, and J. Wu, X. Guo, X. Xu, N. Wang and L. Wang for assistance with field work.

  • Crowe TM. 1978a. Limitations of population in the Helmeted Guineafowl. S Afr J Wildl Res, 8: 117–126
    Crowe TM. 1978b. The evolution of guinea fowl (Galliformes, Phasianidae, Numidinae). Ann S Afr Museum, 76: 43–136
    Crowe TM, Crowe AA. 1979. Anatomy of the vascular system of the head and neck of the helmeted guineafowl Numida meleagris. J Zool, 188: 221–233
    Crowe TM, Elbin SB. 1987. Social behaviour of Helmeted Guineafowl Numida meleagris. S Afr J Wildl Res Suppl, 1: 55–57
    Elbin SB. 1979. Social organization in a group of free-ranging Domestic Guineafowl. MSc dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, Pennsylvania
    Elbin SB, Crowe TM, Graves HB. 1986. Reproductive behaviour of Helmeted Guinea Fowl (Numida meleagris): mating system and parental care. Appl Anim Behav Sci, 16: 179–197
    Hockey PAR, Dean WRJ, Ryan PG. 2005. Roberts- Birds of Southern Africa, Ⅶth edn. The Trustees of the Johan Voelckler Bird Book Fund, Cape Town
    Little RM, Crowe TM, Peall SKC. 1997. Pesticide residues in Helmeted Guineafowl Numida meleagris livers collected in deciduous fruit farms in the Western Cape province, South Africa. S Afr J Wildl Res, 27: 1–4
    Maier V. 1982 Acoustic communication in the Guinea Fowl (Numida meleagris): structures and use of vocalizations, and the principles of message coding. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie, 59: 29–83
    Mentis MT, Poggenpoel B, Maguire RRK. 1975. Food of Helmeted Guineafowl in highland Natal. J S Afr Wildl Assoc, 5: 23–25
    Pero LV, Crowe TM. 1996. Helmeted Guineafowl Numida meleagris in KwaZulu-Natal: a case for non-sustainability. S Afr J Wildl Res, 26: 123–130
    Prinsloo HC, Harley V, Reilly BK, Crowe TM. 2005. Sex-related variation in morphology of helmeted guineafowl (Numida meleagris) from Riemland of the northeastern Free State. S Afr J Wildl Res, 35: 96–98
    Ratcliffe CS, Crowe TM. 2001. Habitat utilization and home range size of Helmeted Guineafowl (Numida meleagris) in the Midlands of KwaZulu-Natal province, South Africa. Biol Conserv, 98: 333–345
    Siegfried WR. 1966. Growth, plumage development and moult in the Crowned Helmeted Guineafowl, Numida meleagris coronata Gurney, Department of Nature Conservation Investigation Report, 8: 1–52
    Siopes TD, Wilson WO. 1973. Determination of the sex of Chukar Partridge at hatching. J Wildl Manag, 37: 239–240
    Skead CJ. 1962. A study of the Crowned Guinea Fowl Numida meleagris coronata Gurney. Ostrich, 33: 51–65
    Treisman M. 1975. Predation and the evolution of gregariousness. I Models for concealment and evasion. Anim Behav, 23: 779–800
    Van Niekerk JH. 1979. Social and breeding behaviour of the Crowned Guineafowl in the Krugersdorp Game Reserve. Ostrich, 50: 188–189
    Van Niekerk JH. 1980. Some socio-biological features of Crowned Guineafowl in the Krugersdorp Game Reserve. Bokmakierie, 32: 102–108
    Van Niekerk JH. 1983. Marking and observing Helmeted Guineafowl in the Krugersdorp game reserve. Safring News, 12(2): 48–52
    Van Niekerk JH. 1985. Submissive display in young Helmeted Guineafowl. S Afr J Zool, 20: 38
    Van Niekerk JH. 2001. Social and breeding behaviour of Crested Francolin in the Rustenburg district, South Africa. S Afr J Wildl Res, 31(1): 35–42
    Van Niekerk JH. 2002. Notes on habitat use by Helmeted Guineafowl in the Krugersdorp Game Reserve, South Africa. S Afr J Wildl Res, 32: 166–169
    Van Niekerk JH, Van Ginkel CM. 2003. Notes on the behavioural ecology of Coqui Francolin in the Rustenburg district, South Africa. S Afr J Wildl Res, 33(1): 59–62
    Van Niekerk JH, Barendse M, Mare F. 2009. Behaviour of Red-necked Spurfowl Pternistis afer in the Boknes and Cannon Rock coastal resorts, Alexandria district, Eastern Cape province, South Africa. Ostrich, 80: 43–45
    Van Niekerk JH. 2009. Interflock movements in a population of Helmeted Guineafowl Numida meleagris at the Krugersdorp Game Reserve, Gauteng province, South Africa. Ostrich, 80: 201–204
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(2)  /  Tables(4)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (3777) PDF downloads (3800) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return