Jeffery D. Sullivan, Paul R. Marbán, Jennifer M. Mullinax, David F. Brinker, Peter C. McGowan, Carl R. Callahan, Diann J. Prosser. 2020: Assessing nest attentiveness of Common Terns via video cameras and temperature loggers. Avian Research, 11(1): 22. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-020-00208-7
Citation: Jeffery D. Sullivan, Paul R. Marbán, Jennifer M. Mullinax, David F. Brinker, Peter C. McGowan, Carl R. Callahan, Diann J. Prosser. 2020: Assessing nest attentiveness of Common Terns via video cameras and temperature loggers. Avian Research, 11(1): 22. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-020-00208-7

Assessing nest attentiveness of Common Terns via video cameras and temperature loggers

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Diann J. Prosser, dprosser@usgs.gov

  • Received Date: 30 Mar 2020
  • Accepted Date: 03 Jul 2020
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 07 Jul 2020
  • Background 

    While nest attentiveness plays a critical role in the reproductive success of avian species, nest attentiveness data with high temporal resolution is not available for many species. However, improvements in both video monitoring and temperature logging devices present an opportunity to increase our understanding of this aspect of avian behavior.

    Methods 

    To investigate nest attentiveness behaviors and evaluate these technologies, we monitored 13 nests across two Common Tern (Sterna hirundo) breeding colonies with a paired video camera - temperature logger approach, while monitoring 63 additional nests with temperature loggers alone. Observations occurred from May to August of 2017 on Poplar (Chesapeake Bay, Maryland, USA) and Skimmer Islands (Isle of Wight Bay, Maryland, USA). We examined data respective to four times of day: Morning (civil dawn‒11:59), Peak (12:00‒16:00), Cooling (16:01‒civil dusk), and Night (civil dusk‒civil dawn).

    Results 

    While successful nests had mostly short duration off-bouts and maintained consistent nest attentiveness throughout the day, failed nests had dramatic reductions in nest attentiveness during the Cooling and Night periods (p < 0.05) with one colony experiencing repeated nocturnal abandonment due to predation pressure from a Great Horned Owl (Bubo virginianus). Incubation appeared to ameliorate ambient temperatures during Night, as nests were significantly warmer during Night when birds were on versus off the nest (p < 0.05). Meanwhile, off-bouts during the Peak period occurred during higher ambient temperatures, perhaps due to adults leaving the nest during the hottest periods to perform belly soaking. Unfortunately, temperature logger data alone had limited ability to predict nest attentiveness status during shorter bouts, with results highly dependent on time of day and bout duration. While our methods did not affect hatching success (p > 0.05), video-monitored nests did have significantly lower clutch sizes (p < 0.05).

    Conclusions 

    The paired use of iButtons and video cameras enabled a detailed description of the incubation behavior of COTE. However, while promising for future research, the logistical and potential biological complications involved in the use of these methods suggest that careful planning is needed before these devices are utilized to ensure data is collected in a safe and successful manner.

  • Avian reproduction has a series of complex behaviors, such as mate selection, nest site selection, egg laying, incubation, and nestling feeding (Zheng 2012). These behaviors are a sequence of adaptations formed through natural selection that are beneficial to individual and population survival (Saether and Bakke 2000). There are about 10, 000 bird species (del Hoyo et al. 2013). The fundamental breeding information (nesting, incubation and nestling periods) of only about one-third of these species is known while the rest are poorly understood (Xiao et al. 2017). Passerines have the greatest number of species (6004) and inhabit all continents except Antarctica (Sibley et al.1991; del Hoyo et al. 2013). Fewer than 7% of studies on passerines include breeding data (Pienaar et al. 2013). The tropics contain 73% of all bird species but breeding information have been well documented in only 20.2% of tropic bird species, compared to 57.9% of bird species in temperate areas (Kricher 2001; Levy 2008; Xiao et al. 2017). It is difficult to study the natural habitats and nests of birds in tropical forests because of the hot and humid climate and dense vegetation (Xiao et al. 2017). Using artificial nest boxes facilitates studies of breeding biology of birds, especially for secondary cavity-nesting birds. Detailed information of avian reproduction can be obtained when birds accept and use nest boxes. Artificial nest boxes are convenient for breeding ecology studies but are also useful for other studies such as niche utilization (Ye et al. 2019).

    We studied the Yellow-bellied Warbler (Abroscopus superciliaris) (Passeriformes, Cettiidae). There are three species in Abroscopus, including the Yellow-bellied Warbler, Rufous-faced Warbler (A. albogularis) and Black-faced Warbler (A. schisticeps). Knowledge of the breeding ecology of these three warblers is limited and little clutch size information has been reported (Xiao et al. 2017). We studied the breeding ecology of A. superciliaris and documented reproductive information throughout egg and nestling periods. Data collected included nest components, nest size, egg laying date and time, egg morph, egg size, clutch size, egg incubation, nestling brooding and feeding, nestling morph and growth, and reproductive outcome.

    This study was performed in Nonggang village (23°39ʹN, 107°04ʹE), located in Guangxi Province of Southwest China, from April to July 2019. This location has a tropical monsoon climate with an average annual temperature ranging from 20.8–22.4℃ and an average annual rainfall ranging from 1150–1550 mm (Feng et al.2019). A. superciliaris are resident birds mainly distributed in the eastern part of the Himalayas, Southern China and Southeast Asia. They are secondary cavity-nesters with similar morphology between males and females (del Hoyo et al. 2013).

    We established 191 nest boxes to attract A. superciliaris. Nest boxes were made from pine wood and were 30 cm high × 15 cm long and wide, with a 6-cm diameter hole. They were attached to trees at a height of 3 m before the breeding season and inspected regularly for warbler activity. Nests in natural habitat were found by searching for holes in bamboo habitat. We recorded the nest materials, egg laying date, egg morph, clutch size, nestling morph, and measured the egg size, egg mass, nestling tarsus length, and nestling mass during nest inspection. Egg length, egg width, and nestling tarsus length were surveyed by a caliper. Egg mass and nestling mass were surveyed by an electric scale. For the calculation of egg size and mass, to avoid pseudo-replication, the average values from each nest represented one sample rather than using each egg as one sample. Two warbler nests in nest boxes were monitored 24 h continuously from egg laying to egg incubation and nestling feeding stages using a mini-camera (WJO3, Hisilicon, Shenzhen, P.R. China). A total of 77, 760 min of video were recorded from nest T12 and J1. T12 included intact video data of both egg and nestling stages for analyses while for J1 only video data of nestling stage was included because the video data of egg stage was incomplete and excluded from analyses.

    The egg laying date and time, egg incubation and nestling feeding behaviors were extracted by studying the monitoring videos. We summarized incubation rate and time per hour from 6:00 to 19:00 and daily incubation rate and time during egg incubation stage. During the nestling stage, we summarized daily brooding (i.e. a behavior that keeps nestlings warm) frequency and time, feeding frequency per hour from 6:00 to 19:00, and daily feeding frequency. The evaluation of reproductive outcome included calculations of hatching rate, fledging rate, fledging success, and nesting success. Hatching rate was calculated by the proportion of hatching eggs from all eggs while fledging rate was calculated by the proportion of fledglings from all nestlings. Nesting success was defined as the percentage of nests that fledged at least one young and fledging success was calculated as the brood sizes at fledging (Yang et al. 2011). Figures were generated using SigmaPlot 14.0 for Windows (Systat Software Inc., USA). A Student's t test was used to compare the nest sizes in the nest boxes and natural nests. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient was used to test the correlation between incubation rate and time or egg turning frequency. Logistic regression was used to fit the growing curve of nestlings. Data were presented as mean ± SD. Data analyses were performed using IBM SPSS 25.0 for Windows (IBM Inc., USA).

    We found a total of 10 nests of A. superciliaris in nest boxes and 6 nests in natural habitats. Detailed information on breeding ecology was acquired from the nest boxes. We were unable to collect further information except for habitat, nest size, and nest fate because these nests were disturbed by local inhabitants. The nests of A. superciliaris in nest boxes were composed of an awn weaving nest cup surrounded by dry bamboo leaves. The nest cup included some feathers (Fig.1a). In natural habitat A. superciliaris built nests in bamboo holes (hole diameter: 2.5 ± 0.2 cm, n = 6) that were created by the White-browed Piculet (Sasia ochracea) (Fig.1b). The holes were 1.27 ± 0.27 m (n = 6) high. The nest materials were similar to those in the nest boxes but the nest shape differed (Fig.1c). The nest cup diameter was similar between nest box and bamboo nests (nest box: 5.3 ± 1.1 cm, n = 6, bamboo: 5.1 ± 1.5 cm, n = 6; t = -0.359, df = 10, P = 0.727, Student's t-test), but the nest cup depth was different (nest box: 4.2 ± 0.3 cm, n = 6, bamboo: 3.4 ± 0.4 cm, n = 6; t = -3.839, df = 10, P = 0.003, Student's t-test). Clutch size was 4.3 ± 1.2 (range: 2–6, n = 10) and eggs were white in ground color but densely covered with brownish-red markings, which were mostly present on the blunt end (Fig.1a). Egg size and mass were 14.66 ± 0.54 mm × 11.03 ± 0.30 mm and 0.93 ± 0.11 g (n = 7), respectively.

    Figure  1.  Nests and eggs of Yellow-bellied Warbler. a nest and eggs in nest box; b natural nest in bamboo hole; c interior of natural nest which was split after nestlings fledged

    Nest box data showed that the earliest and latest laying dates of the 1st egg were 24 April and 31 May, respectively. A. superciliaris laid one egg each day during egg laying. The laying time of three eggs (3rd, 4th, and 5th egg) from nest T12 was recorded on 6:52, 6:18, and 6:54 in the morning. The egg incubation began after the last egg was laid. Before that, a short-time incubation was also found during the egg laying stage (Fig.2). Although male and female warblers were sexually monomorphic, incubation appeared to be performed solely by the female. The incubation duration and absence intervals showed no signs of alternate incubation. The daily incubation rate was high and the duration was long initially but decreased during the latter part of the egg stage. On the day before the hatch of the first nestling, the incubation time increased to a peak (Fig.2). Incubation rate was positively correlated with incubation time and egg turning frequency (frequency vs time: r = 0.664, P = 0.007; frequency vs turning: r = 0.606, P = 0.017, Spearman's rank correlation coefficient). The incubation time per hour of a day from 6:00 to 19:00 was relatively longer in the morning compared to afternoon, but increased on the last hour (18:00–19:00) (Fig.3). Compared with incubation time, incubation rate was more variable during the incubation period (Fig.3). The egg incubation stage lasted 13 ± 1.7 days (range: 12–15, n = 3), and females remained in the nest boxes at night during the entire incubation stage (701.5 ± 26.3 min/night, n = 12).

    Figure  2.  Breeding behaviors of Yellow-bellied Warbler (T12 nest) in egg and nestling stages. Incubation/brooding frequency refers to the frequency of egg incubation/nestling brooding (keeping eggs and nestlings warm) by the female parent; egg turning frequency refers to the frequency of turning eggs during incubation by female parent
    Figure  3.  Incubation and feeding behaviors of Yellow-bellied Warbler on daytime (6:00–19:00). Data are presented as mean ± SE. Incubation and feeding refer to the behaviors of egg incubation and nestling feeding by parents

    The hatching time of two nestlings in nest T12 was 6:55 and 6:26 on the mornings of two successive days. For nest J1, the hatching time of two nestlings was 11:35 and 12:55 on the same day. Eggshells were ejected by females after nestlings hatched. Females sat on the nest after nestlings hatched to keep the nestling warm (i.e., nestling brooding). The brooding time lasted 11 days, which accounted for 68.75% of nestling stage. It was high on the first 3 days, but decreased rapidly by the 6th day and was absent on the last 5 days (Fig.2). The female parent remained in the nest box at night (649.9 ± 37.8 min/night, n = 10) until the end of nestling brooding (Fig.4). Both males and females participated in nestling feeding. The feeding frequencies of T12 and J1 were similar. They increased gradually with nestling growth but decreased on the last 2 days (Fig.4). The feeding frequency per hour of a day from 6:00 to 19:00 had minor fluctuations (Fig.3).

    Figure  4.  Feeding frequency and night staying time in the nestling stage of the Yellow-bellied Warbler. Feeding frequency refers to the frequency of nestling feeding by parents. Night staying time refers to the time that female parents stayed on the nests

    The growth curves of tarsus length and body mass in nestlings were fitted by logistic regression (Fig.5). Hatchlings on 0 d were naked with some fluffy down on the head. Their skin was a reddish color on 0 d but was yellowish from 1 d forward (Fig.6). Feather buds appeared on the forelegs and the center of the back at 4 d and began to grow within capsules. From 13 d the wing feathers had emerged from the capsules and at the time of fledging the nestling was similar in color to that of adult bird with a white brow and yellow belly (Fig.6). The nestling stage lasted 13.7 ± 1.5 days and ranged from 12 to 15 days (n = 3).

    Figure  5.  Growth of tarsus length and body mass in nestlings fitted by logistic regression
    Figure  6.  Nestlings during growth and adult Yellow-bellied Warblers

    In the 10 nest box nests, the hatching and fledging rates of A. superciliaris were 25.58% (11 out of 43) and 54.55% (6 out of 11), respectively. Occupation by White-rumped Shama (Kittacincla malabarica), predation, and desertion led to nest failure. The nesting success was 40%, with an average fledging success of 1.1 fledglings per nest. All nests from bamboo holes successfully fledged, with a nesting success of 100%, but other information including eggs and nestlings were not studied.

    The incubation time of A. superciliaris gradually decreased during the latter part of the egg stage, but significantly increased on the day before the first nestling hatched (Fig.2). Female parents spent much more time in the nest immediately before the egg hatch. The parent could perceive that the eggs were near hatch. Egg embryos are sensitive to ambient temperature but tolerance to temperature can vary during different development stages (Webb 1987). The parent may modify its incubating behavior according to the development of the egg embryo. Several studies have documented parent-embryo communication in birds (Colombelli-Négrel et al. 2012; Mariette and Buhanan 2016; Katsis et al. 2018; Mariette et al. 2018). It is possible that A. superciliaris parents can either perceive the progress of embryo development or communicate with the embryo to achieve synchronism. Further studies are needed to test these hypotheses.

    According to the allocation of incubation time per hour during the day, females invested more time in the morning than in the afternoon (Fig.3). This suggests that parental foraging time mainly occurred in the afternoon. Birds balance their own survival and reproduction during breeding season (Dammhahn et al. 2018); therefore, such pattern of time allocation may be the result of tradeoff between parent survival and embryo development. Allocating more time for foraging in the afternoon is reasonable because the temperature may be warmer for egg embryos than in the morning. Although the incubation time was similar between some time intervals, the incubation rate was not necessarily consistent. For example, the incubation time between 6:00–7:00 and 18:00–19:00 was similar, but the incubation rate of the former was lower than the latter (Fig.3). This suggests that the parent can maintain the incubation time by modifying the incubation rate. The parent may allocate more time per incubation during 6:00–7:00 than during 18:00–19:00, but maintain a similar total incubation time. This is reasonable because during the last hour before nightfall, parents may come out more frequently to search for food.

    This study provides information on the breeding ecology of A. superciliaris. The breeding ecology of the other two species in Abroscopus is poorly known. According to the phylogeny of old-world warblers, the most closely related warbler clades are Cettia, Tesia, and Urosphena (Alström et al. 2011; Jetz et al. 2012). However, the breeding ecology of only two Cettia warblers (C. diphone and C. cetti) is known (Xiao et al. 2017). Unlike A. superciliaris, these warblers generally built nests in shrubby vegetation rather than in cavities (del Hoyo et al. 2013). For Cettia warblers, egg incubation and nestling feeding are solely performed by females (Kenzo and Takeshi 1970; Bibby 1982; Tasinazzo 1993; del Hoyo et al. 2013), In A. superciliaris males did not incubate eggs but they did participate in nestling feeding. Further studies on the breeding ecology of the other two Abroscopus species might help us to understand their evolution in relation to A. superciliaris.

    We thank anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We thank Accdon for linguistic assistance during manuscript preparation.

    CY conceived and designed the study. JB and YJ conducted the experiments in the field. JB and CY performed the data analyses and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors. The experiments comply with the current laws of China.

    Not applicable.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

  • AlRashidi M. The challenge of coping in an extremely hot environment: a case study of the incubation of Lesser Crested Terns (Thalasseus bengalensis). Waterbirds. 2016;39:215–21.
    Amat JA, Gomez J, Linan-Cembrano G, Rendon MA, Ramo C. Incubating terns modify risk-taking according to diurnal variations in egg camouflage and ambient temperature. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2017;71:72.
    Antolos M, Roby DD, Lyons DE, Anderson SK, Collis K. Effects of nest density, location, and timing on breeding success of Caspian Terns. Waterbirds. 2006;29:465–72.
    Arnold JM, Saboom D, Nisbet ICT, Hatch JJ. Use of temperature sensors to monitor patterns of nocturnal desertion by incubating Common Terns. J Field Ornithol. 2006;77:384–91.
    Arnold JM, Hatch JJ, Nisbet ITC. Seasonal declines in reproductive success of the common tern Sterna hirundo: timing or parental quality? J Avian Biol. 2008;35:33–45.
    Bollinger PB, Bollinger EK, Malecki RA. Tests of three hypotheses of hatching asynchrony in the Common Tern. Auk. 1990;107:696–706.
    Bonter DN, Bridge ES. Applications of radio frequency identification (RFID) in ornithological research: a review. J Field Ornithol. 2011;82:1–10.
    Burger J, Gochfeld M. The Common Tern: Its breeding biology and social behavior. New York: Columbia University Press; 1991.
    Clode D, Birks JDS, Macdonald DW. The influence of risk and vulnerability on predator mobbing by terns (Sterna spp.) and gulls (Larus spp.). J Zool. 2000;252:53–9.
    Courtney P. Seasonal variation in intra-clutch hatching intervals among Common Terns Sterna hirundo. Ibis. 1979;121:207–11.
    Deeming DC, Reynolds SJ. Nests, eggs, and incubation: new ideas about avian reproduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2015.
    Gotmark F, Andersson M. Colonial breeding reduces nest predation in the Common Gull (Larus canus). Anim Behav. 1984;32:485–92.
    Grant GS. Foot-wetting and belly-soaking by incubating Gull-billed Terns and Black Skimmers. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc. 1978;75:148–52.
    Grant GS. Belly-soaking by incubating common, sandwich, and royal terns. J Field Ornithol. 1981;52:244.
    Grant GS. Avian incubation: egg temperature, nest humidity, and behavioral thermoregulation in a hot environment. Ornithol Monogr. 1982;30:ⅲ.
    Hand JL, Hunt GL Jr, Warner M. Thermal stress and predation: influences on the colony structure of a gull colony and possibly breeding distributions. Condor. 1981;83:193–203.
    Hart LA, Downs CT, Brown M. Sitting in the sun: nest microhabitat affects incubation temperatures in seabirds. J Therm Biol. 2016;60:149–54.
    Hartman CA, Oring LW. An inexpensive method for remotely monitoring nest activity. J Field Ornithol. 2006;77:418–24.
    Hays H, LeCroy M. Field Criteria for determining incubation stage in eggs of the Common Tern. Wilson Bull. 1971;83:425–9.
    Hébert PN. Breeding failure and decline of a Common Tern colony in southern Manitoba. Col Waterbirds. 1985;8:183–5.
    Hunt GL Jr, Hunt MW. Reproductive ecology of the Western Gull: the importance of nest spacing. Auk. 1975;92:270–9.
    Kar T, Debata S. Breeding ecology of the endangered Black-Bellied Tern (Sterna acuticauda) in eastern India and implications for conservation. Waterbirds. 2019;42:314–20.
    Konishi S, Kitagawa G. Information criteria and statistical modeling. New York: Springer; 2008.
    Mallory ML. Reactions of ground-nesting marine birds to human disturbance in the Canadian Arctic. Arct Sci. 2016;2:67–77.
    Marshall N. Night desertion by nesting Common Terns. Wilson Bull. 1942;54:25–31.
    Martin TE. Food as a limit on breeding birds: a life-history perspective. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1987;18:453–87.
    Martin TE. A new view of avian life-history evolution tested on an incubation paradox. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2002;269:309–16.
    Maryland Department of Natural Resources. Maryland's Natural Areas: Skimmer Island, Worchester County. 2016. .
    Maryland Environmental Service. About Poplar Island. 2017. .
    Meehan TD, Nisbet ICT. Nest attentiveness in Common Terns threatened by a model predator. Waterbirds. 2002;25:278–84.
    Morris RD, Hunter RA. Monitoring incubation attentiveness of ground-nesting colonial seabirds. J Wildl Manage. 1976;40:354–7.
    Morris RD, Hunter RA, McElman JF. Factors affecting the reproductive success of Common Tern (Sterna hirundo) colonies on the lower Great Lakes during the summer of 1972. Can J Zool. 1976;54:1850–62.
    Neumann JL, Larose CS, Brodin G, Feare CJ. Foraging ranges of incubating Sooty Terns Onychoprion fuscatus on Bird Island, Seychelles, during a transition from food plenty to scarcity, as revealed by GPS loggers. Mar Ornithol. 2018;46:11–8.
    Nisbet ICT. Belly-soaking by incubating and brooding Common Terns. J Field Ornithol. 1983;54:190–2.
    Nisbet ICT, Cohen ME. Asynchronous hatching in Common and Roseate Terns Sterna hirundo and S. dougallii. Ibis. 1975;117:374–9.
    Nisbet ICT, Arnold JM, Oswald SA, Pyle P, Patten MA. Common Tern (Sterna hirundo). In: The Birds of North America. Cornell Lab of Ornithology. 2017. .
    Nisbet ICT, Welton MJ. Seasonal variation in breeding success of Common Terns: consequences of predation. Condor. 1984;86:53–60.
    NOAA National Centers for Environmental Information. 2018. Climate Data Online. .
    Nordstrom M, Laine J, Ahola M, Korpimaki E. Reduced nest defence intensity and improved breeding success in terns as responses to removal of nonnative American mink. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004;55:454–60.
    Norwood GJ. Nest-site selection, nocturnal nest desertion, and productivity in a Common Tern (Sterna hirundo) colony at Detroit River, Michigan. Master's Thesis. Ypsilanti, MI: Eastern Michigan University. 2011.
    Palestis BG. Nesting stage and nest defense by Common Terns. Waterbirds. 2005;28:87–94.
    R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2018. .
    Richardson TW, Gardali T, Jenkins SH. Review and meta-analysis of camera effects on avian nest success. J Wildl Manage. 2009;73:287–93.
    Riechert J, Becker PH. What makes a good parent? Sex-specific relationships between nest attendance, hormone levels, and breeding success in a long-lived seabird. Auk. 2017;134:644–58.
    Schneider EG, McWilliams SR. Using nest temperature to estimate nest attendance of Piping Plovers. J Wildl Manage. 2007;71:1998–2006.
    Seefelt NE, Farrell PD. Indirect negative impacts of Double-crested Cormorant (Palacrocorax auritus) management on co-nesting Caspain Terns (Hydroprogne caspia) in Northern Lake, Michigan, USA. Waterbirds. 2018;41:417–23.
    Shaffer SA, Clatterbuck CA, Kelsey EC, Naiman AD, Young LC, VanderWerf EA, et al. As the egg turns: monitoring egg attendance behavior in wild birds using novel data logging technology. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e97898.
    Shealer DA, Kress SW. Nocturnal abandonment response to Black-Crowned Night-Heron disturbance in a Common Tern colony. Col Waterbirds. 1991;14:51–6.
    Smithson M, Verkuilen J. A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychol Methods. 2006;11:54–71.
    Sullivan JD, Marban PR, Mullinax JM, Brinker DF, McGowan PC, Callahan CR, et al. Assessing nest attentiveness of Common Terns (Sterna hirundo) via video cameras and temperature loggers. 2019. U.S. Geological Survey data release, .
    Taylor GT, Ackerman JT, Shaffer SA. Egg turning behavior and incubation temperature in Forester's terns in relation to mercury contamination. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0191390.
    U.S. Navy Observatory. Sun or Moon Rise/Set Table for One Year. 2016. .
    Vedder O, Kurten N, Bouwhuis S. Interspecific variation in and environmentdependent resource allocation to embryonic development time in Common Terns. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2017;4:453–60.
    Wall JW, Marban PR, Brinker DF, Sullivan JD, Zimnik M, Murrow JL, et al. A video surveillance system to monitor breeding colonies of Common Terns (Sterna hirundo). J Vis Exp. 2018;137:e57928.
    Weathers WW, Zaun BJ. Egg-turning behavior and nest attentiveness of the endangered Hawaiian Goose on Kauai. West Birds. 2010;41:2–9.
    Webb DR, King JR. An analysis of the heat budgets of the eggs and nest of the White-Crowned Sparrow, Zonotrichia leucophrys, in relation to parental attentiveness. Physiol Zool. 1983;56:493–505.
    Wendeln H, Becker PH. Does distrubance by nocturnal predators affect body mass of adult Common Terns? Waterbirds. 1999;22:401–10.
    Wiggins DA, Morris RD. Parental care of the Common Tern Sterna hirundo. Ibis. 1987;129:533–40.
    Yoon J, Yoon H, Go B, Joo E, Park S. Tide associated incubation and foraging behavior of Saunders's Gulls Larus saundersi. Ardea. 2014;101:99–104.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(4)

    1. Ziqi ZHANG, Qihong LI, Yan CAI, et al. Revealing the key signals in nestling begging behavior perceived by parent birds during parent–offspring conflict. Integrative Zoology, 2024. DOI:10.1111/1749-4877.12901
    2. Ziqi Zhang, Jianli Bi, Xu Zhao, et al. Comparison of Reproductive Strategies between Two Sympatric Copsychus Passerines. Animals, 2024, 14(4): 554. DOI:10.3390/ani14040554
    3. Karl T. Heide, Thomas E. Martin. Breeding biology of the Bornean Stubtail (Urosphena whiteheadi) on Mount Kinabalu, Sabah, Malaysia. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 2023, 135(2) DOI:10.1676/22-00078
    4. Qihong Li, Jianli Bi, Jiangwen Wu, et al. Impact of nest sanitation behavior on hosts’ egg rejection: an empirical study and meta-analyses. Current Zoology, 2021, 67(6): 683. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab057

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(7)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (1080) PDF downloads (4) Cited by(4)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return