
Citation: | Muñoz-Pedreros Andrés, González-Urrutia Marilyn, Encina-Montoya Francisco, Norambuena Heraldo V.. 2018: Effects of vegetation strata and human disturbance on bird diversity in green areas in a city in southern Chile. Avian Research, 9(1): 38. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-018-0130-9 |
Urbanisation is a dominant geographical trend and an important component of global change, with unprecedented implications for socio-economic, cultural and environmental characteristics. However, green areas, including original fragments, can help to conserve native diversity, improving the functioning of these artificial systems in the long term. Urban areas can still provide habitats usable by wild birds, however the structural characteristics of the habitat formed by different types of green area differ, and therefore dissimilar bird diversities are to be expected. The object of this study was to characterise the α and β diversities of birds in different green areas and to analyse how diversity relates to ten variables that characterise the habitat.
We studied the green areas in the city of Temuco, southern Chile (Park, Square and Median strips of main streets), evaluating the variables: (a) surface area, (b) vegetation, (c) estimated human impact as the proportions of vegetation and bare soil by area, and the vehicle traffic. The bird assemblage structures were characterised by α (intra-environment) diversity and β diversity (between environments) and the statistical analysis identified the environmental variables related with the presence and abundance of birds. A statistical model was constructed to describe the contribution of the variables to bird diversity.
We found significant differences between the diversity of bird species in the three types of green area. The β showed medium to high similarity between the different study units. There was a negative correlation with bare soil areas; the correlations with vehicle flow, plant structure and tree and shrub cover were not significant, meaning that these variables did not explain the variation in the richness of bird species between the green areas. However the surface area did explain this variation presenting a positive potential relation. There was also a high correlation with the origin (native) of shrub species.
The bird diversity varied significantly according to the type of urban green area. The environmental variables presenting significant correlations with bird diversity were: surface area, native species of shrub stratum, shrub cover, and bare soil area. The best multiple regression model showed that the three most important variables for bird diversity are the surface area of the green area, the cover of the shrub stratum and the presence of native shrub species.
Nest sanitation behavior is the removal of foreign objects from the nest, and is a very common behavior in breeding birds (Alvarez et al. 1976; Moskát et al. 2003; Underwood and Sealy 2006; Guigueno and Sealy 2009, 2017). This behavior can improve the incubation efficiency of the parent birds, reduce physical damage to eggs from foreign objects, lower the chance of egg infection by pathogenic bacteria, and decrease the risk of predation (Guigueno and Sealy 2012). For instance, parent birds usually clear the leaves, gravel, broken eggshells, food debris, nestling feces, and other debris from the nest (Weatherhead 1984; Kemal and Rothstein 1988; Petit et al. 1989; Lang et al. 2002; Evans et al. 2016). Nest sanitation is also prevalent in inter-specific brood parasitism birds, and may be associated with the evolution of egg discrimination in the anti-parasitic strategy of host birds (Rothstein 1975).
Previous work suggested that the evolution of egg rejection in cuckoo hosts is driven by the pressure from cuckoo parasitism; the higher the parasitism pressure, the stronger the egg discrimination ability of the hosts (Davies and Brooke 1989; Moksnes et al. 1991). However, egg rejection by hosts may be an extension of their nest sanitation behavior (Rothstein 1975; Guigueno and Sealy 2009; Yang et al. 2015b). Many studies have shown the relationship between the nest sanitation behavior and egg discrimination ability of cuckoo hosts using various non-ovoid foreign objects (Ortega and Cruz 1988; Moskát et al. 2003; Underwood and Sealy 2006; Yang et al. 2015a, b; Luro and Hauber 2017; Peer 2017; review in Rothstein 1975; Peer and Sealy 2004; Guigueno and Sealy 2017). Moskát et al. (2003) proposed that the less similar the shape of a foreign object to a cuckoo egg, the higher the host rejection rate, so that the ability of the host to recognize a cuckoo egg is an extension of its nest sanitation behavior. In addition, hosts with strong egg discrimination ability tend to be more capable of rejecting non-ovoid objects than are hosts without egg discrimination ability (Yang et al. 2015b), indicating that host egg discrimination ability is associated with nest sanitation behaviour (Underwood and Sealy 2006; Guigueno and Sealy 2009; Yang et al. 2015a). The removal of non-egg-shaped objects is stable across pre-hatching stages, unless precluded by rejection costs (Guigueno and Sealy 2017). Therefore, nest sanitation behaviour may be a preadaptation to egg discrimination behaviour, as hypothesized by Guigueno and Sealy (2012).
In Tree Sparrows (Passer montanus), it seems that nest sanitation plays a key role in the evolution of the removal of parasitic eggs (Poláček et al. 2013; but see Yang et al. 2016). Yang et al. (2015b) showed that the nest sanitation behavior of Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) was a pre-adaptation to identifying the eggs of parasitic cuckoos. They further demonstrated that nest sanitation behavior could significantly improve the ability of parent swallows to reject non-mimetic eggs (Yang et al. 2015a). In contrast to Yang et al. (2015a), the Red-winged Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus), a host of the Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater), was not more likely reject a model cowbird egg, with its maculation but different background colour, if the model egg was simultaneously added to the nest with flagging tape or a pine cone bract scale (Peer 2017). Similarly, whether American Robins (Turdus migratorius) removed a Douglas Fir (Pseudotsuga menziesii) leaf clipping from their nests or not showed no effect on rejection of a model cowbird egg (Luro and Hauber 2017). Therefore, a potential proximate link between nest sanitation and egg ejection in avian brood parasite-host system is still inconclusive (Luro and Hauber 2017).
For research, replicating work is crucial to assess the generality of findings (Nakagawa and Parker 2015). However, the complexity of data collection in ecology, especially in field ecology, often precludes the possibility of going beyond single-population studies (Canestrari et al. 2017). For example, a study by Canestrari et al. (2014) suggested a mechanism based on the malodorous secretion of Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius) chicks to explain why the presence of the parasite in the nests of Carrion Crows (Corvus corone) in northern Spain increased the probability of nest success as compared to non-parasitized nests. However, also in Spain, Soler et al. (2017) found no evidence to support an anti-depredatory function of cuckoo chicks in their studied populations (but see Canestrari et al. 2017). Clearly, more studies addressing whether nest sanitation elicits egg rejection are needed (Guigueno and Sealy 2017).
In this study, we investigated the relationship between nest sanitation and egg rejection in an open-cup nesting potential cuckoo host, the Brown-breasted Bulbul (Pycnonotus xanthorrhous). Although the Brown-breasted Bulbul in China has not been recorded as being parasitized by parasitic cuckoos (Yang et al. 2012; Su et al. 2016), most species of bulbuls were listed as hosts for many cuckoo species (Cuculus spp.) (Davies 2000), and in Africa, Cape Bulbuls (Pycnonotus capensis) and other bulbul species (Pycnonotus spp.) were common hosts of the Jacobin Cuckoo (Clamator jacobinus) (Krüger 2011). Here, we used blue, non-mimetic eggs and peanut half-shells to evaluate whether nest sanitation behavior in Brown-breasted Bulbuls influences their egg discrimination ability following the experimental protocols by Yang et al. (2015a). If nest sanitation behavior is directly associated with egg discrimination ability, then egg discrimination against non-mimetic eggs should be significantly higher in nests with peanut shells than in those without. We predict that nest sanitation behavior would elicit egg rejection in the Brown-breasted Bulbul.
The study was conducted during the breeding season of the Brown-breasted Bulbul (April–August) in 2014–2016 in Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserve (28°10′N, 107°10′E) in Guizhou, Southwest China. Bulbul nests were searched covering all habitats in the study area, and breeding parameters, such as the nesting and hatching dates, were recorded. We estimated the hatching stage following Hays and LeCroy (1971). No cuckoo parasitism was recorded in Brown-breasted Bulbuls although they are abundant in our study area and their nests were easy to find (Yang et al. 2012; Su et al. 2016). We followed the experimental protocols by Yang et al. (2015a) and performed the experiment during the early incubation stage. The blue, non-mimetic eggs were made from baked soft clay, with a mean weight of 4.47 ± 0.16 g, length of 21.42 ± 0.46 mm, and width of 15.99 ± 0.42 mm (n = 15). The mean weight, length, and width of the peanut shells was 1.76 ± 0.45 g, 36.57 ± 4.15 mm, and 15.29 ± 1.38 mm, respectively (n = 15). The mean weight, length, and width of the Brown-breasted Bulbul eggs was 2.92 ± 0.29 g, 21.88 ± 1.02 mm, and 16.35 ± 0.54 mm, respectively (n = 74) (Fig. 1). In the first experimental group, we directly added a non-mimetic blue egg to nests of Brown-breasted Bulbuls to evaluate the ability of the host to discriminate against the non-mimetic egg within 6 days (Davies and Brooke 1989; Moksnes et al. 1991; Yang et al. 2015a). In the second group, we added a peanut half-shell to nests of Brown-breasted Bulbuls in addition to one non-mimetic blue egg. We used Fisher's exact test to compare the probability of rejecting non-mimetic eggs between different experimental groups. Data analysis was performed using IBM SPSS 19.0 software (IBM Inc., USA).
In the first experimental group, 26 nests of Brown-breasted Bulbuls were tested for discrimination ability against a non-mimetic egg. In 14 out of 26 nests, the non-mimetic blue eggs were rejected (53.8%) (Table 1). In the second experimental group, 19 nests were tested for discrimination ability against a non-mimetic egg together with a peanut shell. All the peanut shells were removed within the same day that they were added (19 out of 19 nests, 100%), while the non-mimetic eggs were rejected in 10 out of 19 nests (52.6%) (Table 1). Brown-breasted Bulbuls used grasp ejection to reject peanut shells and/or model eggs with no rejection cost. All egg ejections occurred after the peanut shells were removed and all parent birds rejected the non-mimetic eggs within 3 days. There was no significant difference in the probability of rejecting non-mimetic eggs between the two experimental groups (χ2 = 0.007, df = 1, p = 0.936). In the experimental group with both non-mimetic eggs and peanut shells, the rejection rate of non-mimetic eggs was significantly lower than that of peanut shells (χ2 = 11.79, df = 1, p=0.0006), indicating that Brown-breasted Bulbuls did not treat the non-mimetic eggs as foreign objects.
Experimental group | Peanut shell rejection (%) | Egg rejection (%) | No. of nests tested |
One non-mimetic egg | 14 (53.8) | 26 | |
One non-mimetic egg and one half peanut shell | 19 (100.0) | 10 (52.6) | 19 |
Our results showed that although Brown-breasted Bulbuls have strong nest sanitation abilities, their nest sanitation behavior did not directly affect their recognition of non-mimetic eggs. The egg rejection rates were similar for the nests with and without the addition of peanut shells. In addition, the Brown-breasted Bulbuls did not treat the non-mimetic eggs as foreign objects, such as peanut shells. This was shown in the nests with both peanut shells and non-mimetic eggs, in which all of the birds removed the peanut shells, but only 52.6% rejected the non-mimetic eggs. Therefore, this indicated the influence of the shape of the object on egg discrimination ability, similar to the results of previous studies (Ortega and Cruz 1988; Guigueno and Sealy 2009, 2012).
Nest sanitation behavior is considered a pre-adaptation of host bird egg discrimination ability, as the two behaviors share a common aspect of the removal of objects from the nest, although some nest sanitation behavior is accomplished by burying or swallowing (Guigueno and Sealy 2012). However, the two behaviors differ with respect to the shape and color of the foreign object and the similarity to the host egg. There is also no clear link between nest sanitation in cowbird hosts (Rothstein 1975; Peer and Sealy 2004). In addition, some hosts do not practice nest sanitation, but can reject some eggs (Peer 2017). Underwood and Sealy (2006) suggested that the ability of American Robins and Gray Catbirds (Dumetella carolinensis) to recognize non-egg objects was significantly higher than that of other species without egg discrimination ability.
Moskát et al. (2003) examined the relationship between nest sanitation and egg rejection in the Great Reed Warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) and showed that the rejection rates of the non-egg models were significantly higher than those of cuckoo model eggs, either during hatching or in the early stage of incubation. Guigueno and Sealy (2009) also showed that in American Yellow Warblers (Setophaga petechia) the burial probability of non-egg objects was significantly greater than that of the simulated cowbird-egg-shaped models during the pre-hatching and hatching stages. These results strongly indicated that host egg discrimination ability is likely to be an extension of nest sanitation behavior.
Yang et al. (2015a) studied the nest sanitation and egg rejection of Barn Swallows, which are hosts of the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus). In nests with only non-mimetic eggs, the probability of swallows rejecting the model eggs was 27%, whereas in nests with model eggs and peanut shells, all of the individuals were able to remove the peanut shells and the probability of rejecting the model eggs increased to 56%, demonstrating the significant influence of host bird nest sanitation behavior on egg discrimination. Similar to Barn Swallows, Brown-breasted Bulbuls also showed moderate egg discrimination ability against non-mimetic eggs (64.1%, Su et al. 2016; 53.8%, this study), which were similar to American Robins (65%; Luro and Hauber 2017) but stronger than the discrimination ability of Red-winged Blackbirds (0%; Peer 2017). However, our results showed no effect of nest sanitation behavior on egg discrimination, but were consistent with those by Luro and Hauber (2017) and Peer (2017), who found that although the tested hosts showed nest sanitation behavior, the addition of foreign objects to the nest did not influence host egg rejection. Rothstein (1975) also found that Red-winged Blackbirds show a very low level of response to cowbird eggs, and foreign objects removal did not influence their egg rejection.
Previous studies have shown that when a host bird sees an adult cuckoo around its nest, it is stimulated to increase the probability of rejecting the parasitic eggs in its nest (Moksnes et al. 1993; Davies 2000; Hosoi and Rothstein 2000; Guigueno and Sealy 2011). Yang et al. (2015a) further showed that this type of stimulation was also produced by foreign objects, which increased egg rejection in Barn Swallows. Such type of stimulation was effective in Barn Swallow populations, but not in American Robins, Red-winged Blackbirds or Brown-breasted Bulbuls. We suggested the possible reason for this discrepancy could be that the nesting habitat of Barn Swallows is different from the latter three, as Barn Swallows are a communally nesting species and their nests are usually built under the eaves of human habitation (Liang et al. 2013; Yang et al. 2015b). In such a nesting habitat, there are relatively fewer pieces of debris that fall into swallow nests during the incubation period. In addition, since the swallow nests are next to the eaves and are relatively dark, small pieces of debris may be difficult for the birds to see. As a result, swallows do not frequently clean their nests during incubation. Thus, when peanut shells suddenly appeared as apparent foreign objects in their nests, nest sanitation behavior was activated and their egg discrimination abilities were also stimulated. By contrast, in the natural habitat of the Brown-breasted Bulbuls, American Robins and Red-winged Blackbirds, many pieces of debris often fall into their nests during the incubation period. In addition, as their nests are cup-shaped and open, the fallen small-sized debris is easily seen. Thus, they often clean their nests throughout the incubation period, and nest sanitation behavior is a normal part of their behavior during incubation. When the peanut shells were added to their nests, the additional stimulation effect was not sufficient to be detected or to induce them to reject the non-mimetic eggs. Clearly, large-scale and multiple-population studies, and more in-depth explanation of these results, e.g., the costs of nest sanitation versus egg rejection and the role of host age structure in nest sanitation, need further investigation in the future.
The results of this study showed that nest sanitation behavior is common among Brown-breasted Bulbuls, but the removal of foreign objects from the nest does not have a direct impact on their egg discrimination ability. In other words, nest sanitation behavior of the Brown-breasted Bulbul was not associated with egg discrimination ability, and nest sanitation did not elicit egg rejection in the Brown-breasted Bulbul. In addition, our study highlighted the necessity to test nest sanitation versus egg rejection in more host species and in multiple populations.
WL and SC conceived and designed the study. TS conducted the experiments in the field. CY performed the data analysis. TS wrote the early draft, WL and SC discussed and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
We would like to thank Dr. Ken Cheng and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an early version of this manuscript. We thank Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserve, Guizhou, China, for permission to undertake this study. We are grateful to Juan Huo and Guoxian Liang for their assistance with fieldwork.
The authors declare that they have no competing interests. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Not applicable.
This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors. The experiments comply with the current laws of China. Fieldwork was carried out under permission from Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserve, Guizhou, China. Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University (permit no. HNECEE-2016-003).
Anguita R. Animales en la gran ciudad (Animals in the big city). Ambienta. 2003;20:32-7.
|
Bibby CJ, Burges N, Hill V. Bird census techniques. British trust for ornithology. Royal society for the protection of birds. San Diego: Academic Press; 1993.
|
Blair RB. Birds and butterflies along an urban gradient: surrogate taxa for assessing biodiversity? Ecol Appl. 1999;9:164-70.
|
Camus P. Bosques y tierras despejadas en el período de la conquista de Chile. In: Retamales J, editor. Forest and land cleared in the period of the conquest of Chile. Estudios coloniales II. Santiago de Chile: Editorial Biblioteca Americana; 2002.
|
Clarke K, Gorley R. Plymouth routines in multivariate ecological research. London: Primer-E. Plymouth Marine Laboratory; 2006.
|
Clarke K, Gorley R, Somerfield P, Warwick R. Change in marine communities: an approach to statistical analysis and interpretation. Devon. 3rd ed. Ivybridge: Primer-E Ltd Lutton; 2014.
|
Day TD. Bird species composition and abundance in relation to native plants in urban gardens, Hamilton, New Zealand. Notornis. 1995;42:172-86.
|
Di Castri F, Hajek F. Bioclimatología de Chile. Bioclimatology of Chile. Santiago de Chile: Publicaciones Vicerrectoría Académica; 1976.
|
Faggi AM. The influence of NGOs in preserving green places in Buenos Aires City. In: Breuste JH, Uhlmann O, editors. Urban ecology. Berlin: Springer; 1998.
|
Gajardo R. La vegetación natural de Chile. Clasificación y distribución geográfica. [The natural vegetation of Chile. Classification and geographical distribution]. Santiago de Chile: Editorial Universitaria; 1994.
|
Gantz A, Rau J. Relación entre el tamaño mínimo de fragmentos boscosos y su riqueza de especies de aves en el sur de Chile. An Museo Hist Nat Valp. 1999;24:85-90.
|
Góngora A. Historia de Chile desde su descubrimiento hasta el año 1575. History of Chile since its discovery until 1575. Colección de Historiadores de Chile. Tomo 11, Santiago de Chile: Imprenta del Ferrocarril; 1862.
|
Halffter G, Ezcurra E. ¿Qué es la biodiversidad? In: Halffter G, editor. La diversidad biológica de Iberoamérica I, Volumen Especial. Ciudad de México: Acta Zoológica Mexicana; 1992.
|
Hauenstein E, Latsague M. Evaluación florística y sinecológica del Monumento Natural Cerro Ñielol (IX Región, Chile). Bol Mus Reg Arauc. 1988;3:7-32.
|
Hauenstein E, Leiva L, González M. Los árboles y arbustos de las plazas de Temuco: II Plazas Recabarren y Teodoro Smith. Árb Nues Ami. 1988;1:43-6.
|
Hauenstein E, Leiva L, González M. Los árboles y arbustos de las plazas de Temuco: III Plazas Dagoberto Godoy y Dreves. Árb Nues Ami. 1989;1:49-53.
|
Hauenstein E, Leiva L. Los árboles y arbustos de las plazas de Temuco: I Plaza Aníbal Pinto. Árb Nues Ami. 1987;3:18-20.
|
Hessayon DG. Árboles y arbustos de jardín: manual de cultivo y conservación. Madrid: Editorial Blume; 1999.
|
Hoffmann A. El árbol urbano en Chile. 3rd ed. Santiago de Chile: Ediciones Fundación Claudio Gay; 1999.
|
Hoffmann A. Flora silvestre de Chile zona araucana. 2nd ed. Santiago de Chile: Ediciones Fundación Claudio Gay; 1991.
|
Jokimäki J, Kaisanlahti-Jokimäki M. Spatial similarity of urban bird communities: a multiscale approach. J Biogeogr. 2003;30:1183-93.
|
Leveau CM, Leveau LM. Ensambles de aves en calles arboladas de tres ciudades costeras del sudeste de la Provincia de Buenos Aires, Argentina. El Hornero. 2006;21:25-30.
|
Magurran A. Ecological diversity and its measurement. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1998.
|
Magurran AE, McGill BJ. Biological diversity frontiers in measurement and assessment. Princeton: Oxford University Press; 2011.
|
Manly BF. Randomization, bootstrap and MonteCarlo methods in biology. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall; 1997.
|
Marín M. Lista comentada de las aves de Chile [Annotated list of the birds of Chile]. Santiago de Chile: Lynk Ediciones; 2004.
|
Maurer BA, McGill BJ. Measurement of species diversity. In: Magurran AE, McGill BJ, editors. Frontiers in measurement and assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011.
|
McNeely JA, Miller KR, Reid WV, Mittermeier RA, Werner TB. Conserving the world's biological diversity. Gland: International Union for the Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources, World Resources Institute, Conservation International, WWF US and World Banks; 1990.
|
Miyasako E. Las áreas verdes en el contexto urbano de la ciudad de México. Thesis Universidad de Alicante, Spain. 2009.
|
Morelli F, Benedetti Y, Ibáñez-Álamo JD, Jokimaki J, Mänd R, Tryjanowski P, Møller AP. Evidence of evolutionary homogenization of bird communities in urban environments across Europe. Global Ecol Biogeogr. 2016;25:1284-93.
|
Mueller-Dombois D, Ellenberg H. Aims and methods of vegetation ecology. New York: Wiley; 1974.
|
Nilsson K, Randrup TB. Silvicultura urbana y periurbana [Urban and periurban forestry]. In: CEPAL, editor. Actividades forestales urbanas y periurbanas. Ciudad de México: Editorial CEPAL; 1997. p. 85-104.
|
Rau J, Gantz A. Fragmentación del bosque nativo del sur de Chile: efectos del área y la forma sobre la biodiversidad de aves. Bol Soc Biol Concep. 2001;72:109-19.
|
Romero H, Toledo Órdenes F, Vásquez A. Ecología urbana y gestión ambiental sustentable de las ciudades intermedias chilenas. Ambiente y Desarrollo. 2001;17(4):45-51.
|
Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ. Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research. 3rd ed. New York: WH Freeman; 1995.
|
Sol D, Bartomeus I, González-Lagos C, Pavoine S. Urbanisation and the loss of phylogenetic diversity in birds. Ecol Lett. 2017;20:721-9.
|
Solari ME, Cueto C, Hernández F, Rojas JF, Camus P. Procesos territoriales y bosques en la cuenca del río Valdivia (siglos XVI-XIX) (Processes land and forests in the Valdivia River basin). Rev Geogr Norte Gd. 2011;49:45-62.
|
Solbrig O. Biodiversity: an introduction. In: Solbrig OT, van Emden HM, van Oordt P, editors. Biodiversity and global change. Wallingford: CAB International; 1994.
|
Tykac J. Arbustos ornamentales. Madrid: Ediciones Susaeta; 1993.
|
Urquiza A, Mella J. Riqueza y diversidad de aves en parques de Santiago durante el período estival. Bol Chil Ornitol. 2002;9:12-21.
|
Vejrup P. Caracterización del sur de la ciudad de Buenos Aires, Argentina [Characterization of Southern Buenos Aires, Argentina]. Thesis. Universidad de Flores, Argentina. 2004.
|
Verner J, Milne KA. Coping with sources of variability when monitoring population trends. Ann Zool Fenn. 1989;26:191-9.
|
de Vivar J. Crónica y relación copiosa y verdadera de los Reinos de Chile (1558) [Chronicle and abundant and true relation of the Kingdoms of Chile]. Berlin: Edición de Leopoldo Sáez Godoy, Biblioteca Ibero-americana, Berlin Colloquium Verlag; 1979.
|
Willson M, Armesto J. Efectos de la fragmentación de bosques para las aves de los bosques australes chilenos. Ambiente y Desarrollo. 2003;19:54-9.
|
Zar JH. Biostatistical analysis. 5th ed. New Jersey: Prentice Hall Inc; 2010.
|
1. | Guillaume Dillenseger. Active defence mechanisms in brood parasitism hosts and their consequences for parasite adaptation and speciation. Journal of Avian Biology, 2024. DOI:10.1111/jav.03252 | |
2. | Jinmei Liu, Fangfang Zhang, Yuran Liu, et al. Egg recognition and nestling discrimination in the Crested Myna (Acridotheres cristatellus): Size matters. Avian Research, 2023, 14: 100111. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100111 | |
3. | Michal Šulc, Anna E. Hughes, Lisandrina Mari, et al. Nest sanitation as an effective defence against brood parasitism. Animal Cognition, 2022, 25(4): 991. DOI:10.1007/s10071-022-01646-0 | |
4. | Qiuhui Yang, Xiangyang Chen, Ziqi Zhang, et al. Egg Rejection and Nest Sanitation in an Island Population of Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica): Probability, Response Latency, and Sex Effects. Animals, 2022, 12(21): 3027. DOI:10.3390/ani12213027 | |
5. | Daniel U. Greene, Robert A. McCleery. Incubation of a foreign object by a Common Nighthawk (Chordeiles minor) in Florida. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 2021, 132(2) DOI:10.1676/1559-4491-132.2.436 | |
6. | Chenyang Liu, Ping Ye, Yan Cai, et al. Persistent fine-tuning of egg rejection based on parasitic timing in a cuckoo host even after relaxation of parasitism pressure. Behavioural Processes, 2021, 193: 104532. DOI:10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104532 | |
7. | Mark E. Hauber, Sarah K. Winnicki, Jeffrey P. Hoover, et al. The limits of egg recognition: testing acceptance thresholds of American robins in response to decreasingly egg-shaped objects in the nest. Royal Society Open Science, 2021, 8(1): 201615. DOI:10.1098/rsos.201615 | |
8. | James B Stratton, Donald C Dearborn, Can-Chao Yang (杨灿朝). Nest sanitation behavior does not increase the likelihood of parasitic egg rejection in herring gulls. Current Zoology, 2021, 67(6): 675. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab046 | |
9. | Qihong Li, Jianli Bi, Jiangwen Wu, et al. Impact of nest sanitation behavior on hosts’ egg rejection: an empirical study and meta-analyses. Current Zoology, 2021, 67(6): 683. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab057 | |
10. | Canchao Yang. Personality, recognition cues, and nest sanitation in obligate avian brood parasitism: what do we know and what comes next?. Current Zoology, 2021, 67(6): 621. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab079 | |
11. | Canchao Yang, Wei Liang, Anders Pape Møller. Using 3D modelling and printing to study avian cognition from different geometric dimensions. Royal Society Open Science, 2019, 6(5): 181938. DOI:10.1098/rsos.181938 |
Experimental group | Peanut shell rejection (%) | Egg rejection (%) | No. of nests tested |
One non-mimetic egg | 14 (53.8) | 26 | |
One non-mimetic egg and one half peanut shell | 19 (100.0) | 10 (52.6) | 19 |