Spencer G Sealy, Todd J Underwood. 2012: Egg discrimination by hosts and obligate brood parasites: a historical perspective and new synthesis. Avian Research, 3(4): 274-294. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2012.0042
Citation: Spencer G Sealy, Todd J Underwood. 2012: Egg discrimination by hosts and obligate brood parasites: a historical perspective and new synthesis. Avian Research, 3(4): 274-294. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2012.0042

Egg discrimination by hosts and obligate brood parasites: a historical perspective and new synthesis

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Spencer G. Sealy, E-mail: sgsealy@cc.umanitoba.ca

  • Received Date: 29 Nov 2012
  • Accepted Date: 18 Dec 2012
  • Available Online: 23 Apr 2023
  • With the knowledge that cuckoos and cowbirds lay their eggs parasitically, and that some hosts eject parasitic eggs, ornithologists began to ponder the question of how host females discriminate between a foreign egg and their own eggs, wondering how hosts "know" which egg to remove. Results of one of the first uncontrolled experiments were inappropriately interpreted to imply ejection was based on discordancy, with hosts simply ejecting the egg in the minority, or the "odd-looking" egg. Controlled experiments eventually revealed that hosts first learn the appearance of own their eggs and discriminate between them and any odd egg in their nest, regardless of which egg type is in the minority. Recent work has shown that discordancy may play a role in discrimination by males mated successively with females that lay polymorphic eggs. We examine the details of the early experiments, in light of recent advances in studies of egg recognition. An ability to recognize eggs also has been extended, implicitly, to include obligate brood parasites, as it underlies several hypotheses in explanation of the behavior of parasites toward their hosts. Egg recognition in parasites, however, has not been experimentally confirmed, nor has a mechanism been identified by which parasites could discriminate between their own eggs and the other eggs in a nest. We review hypotheses (parasite competition, egg removal and multiple parasitism, mafia, farming) that require the ability of obligate brood parasites to discriminate eggs at different levels and the potential mechanisms used by parasites to recognize their own eggs and suggest experiments to test for egg discrimination. An assessment of the egg recognition ability of parasites is germane to our understanding of how parasites counteract defenses of hosts.

  • Pollution of the natural environment by heavy metals is a worldwide problem. Heavy metals enter the aquatic ecosystem through a variety of anthropogenic sources as well as from natural processes (Ebrahimpour and Mushrifah, 2010). They are a serious threat because of their toxicity, bioaccumulation, long persistence and bio-magnification in the food chain (Erdoĝrul and Ates, 2006). The degree of toxic metal uptake, translocation and eventual detoxification within an organism depends on metal speciation, but also differs strongly among organisms (Mukherjee and Nuorteva, 1994; Doyle and Otte, 1997). Assessing ecosystem health adequately by means of biomonitoring requires the selection of representative indicator species. Birds are widely used to biomonitoring variation in environmental levels of anthropogenic pollutants (Furness and Camphuysen, 1997; Barbieri et al., 2009), because they are exposed to a wide range of chemicals and occupy high trophic levels and can therefore provide information on the extent of contamination in the entire food chain (Furness and Camphuysen, 1997; Burger et al., 2007).

    According to Burger and Gochfeld(2000a, 2000b) feathers are useful for measuring heavy metal contamination in birds because birds sequester heavy metals in their feathers, where the proportion of the body burden is relatively constant for each metal. In general, metals in breast feathers are representative of circulating concentrations in the blood stream only during the limited time period of feather formation, which in turn represents both local exposure and mobilization from internal tissues (Lewis and Furness, 1991; Monteiro, 1996). Many studies have recommended herons and egrets as bioindicators for heavy metals in aquatic systems and local pollution around breeding sites (Boncompagni et al., 2003; Kim and Koo, 2007). Herons and gulls are high at the top of their food pyramid and can yield information over a large area around each sampling site, not only on bioavailability of contaminants but also on how, where, and when they are transferred within the food web (Battaglia et al., 2005). Hence, the aim of the present study was to investigate the level of nickel in feathers of two bird species, the Western Reef Heron (Egretta gularis) and the Siberian Gull (Larus heuglini), in order 1) to compare metal concentrations between two species with their life strategy and 2) to examine the species and gender related variation in trace nickel accumulations in the Hara Biosphere Reserve of southern Iran.

    The Hara Biosphere Reserve (26°40′–27°N, 55°21′–55°52′E) is located in southern Iran, in the Straits of Khuran between Queshm Island and the Persian Gulf (Fig. 1). This area became part of the Man and Biosphere Program (MAB) of UNESCO in 1977 (UNESCO, 2010). As well, this region is one of the protected areas in Iran introduced by the Department of the Environment. The entire region was selected as wetland of international importance under the Wetlands of International Importance category as a Habitat of Aquatic Birds. This region was also introduced as one of the important bird areas by the International Organization of Birdlife (Neinavaz et al., 2010).

    Figure  1.  Location of the Hara Biosphere Reserve in southern Iran

    During November and December 2010, under license of the Environmental Protection Agency of Hormozgan, a total of thirty birds were shot and removed from throughout the Hara biosphere reserve. The collection included the Western Reef Heron (Egretta gularis) (n = 15) and the Siberian Gull (Larus heuglini) (n = 15). The birds were transported to the laboratory packed in ice. The specimen were killed, weighed, stored in plastic bags and kept at −20℃ until dissection and analysis. We chose breast feathers because they are representatives of the plumage and are less affected by molt compared to flight feathers. All feathers were analyzed in the Laboratory of the Inland Water Aquaculture Research Institute in port Anzali. The feather samples were digested in a nitric acid (HNO3) and perchloric acid (HClO4) mixture. Feathers were then accurately weighed in 150-mL Erlenmeyer flasks, where 10 mL nitric acid (65%) was added to each sample. The samples were left overnight to be slowly digested; thereafter, 5 mL perchloric acid (70%) was added to each sample. Digestion was performed on a hot plate (sand bath) at 200℃. After that, the digested samples were diluted by 25 mL deionized water. The concentration of nickel was estimated using a Shimadzu AA 680 flame atomic absorption spectrophotometer. The accuracy of the analysis was checked by measuring CRM certified reference tissue (DORM-2, NRC Canada). The detection limit for nickel was 0.039 µg·g−1. The results for nickel gave a mean recovery of 98.6%.

    A statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS (version 18.0). We used a three-way ANOVA for nickel (sex, age, species, interaction (sex × age × species)). Data were log transformed to obtain normal distributions that satisfied the homogeneity of variance required by ANOVA (Custer et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2009). The nickel concentration in feathers was tested for mean differences between species using Student t tests. The level of significance was set at α = 0.05. The concentration of nickel in feathers was expressed in microgram per gram of dry weight (dw). Values are given in means ± standard errors (SE).

    Variations in the nickel concentration of feathers of the Western Reef Heron and Siberian Gull are presented in Table 1. The results show that there was a significant difference between the mean nickel concentrations in the two bird species, i.e. the Western Reef Heron and Siberian Gull, while there was no evidence of significant different accumulation between genders and ages (Table 2). Also, the results indicate that the level of nickel concentration in the Western Reef Heron was higher in females than in males; in contrast, the level of nickel concentration in the Siberian gull was higher in males.

    Table  1.  Geometric means (95% CI) nickel concentrations in feathers of Western Reef Heron and Siberian Gull from the Hara Biosphere Reserve of southern Iran
    Species Male/adult Male/juvenile Female/adult Female/juvenile
    Western Reef Heron
    Geometric mean 2.77 3.79 4.67
    Mean ± SE 3.47±2.7 4.67±2.7 4.67
    Number 9/9 5/5 1
    Siberian Gull
    Geometric mean 8.89 3.87 6.72 7.24
    Mean ± SE 8.94±1.1 4.38±2.5 7.6±3.4 8.1±4.8
    Number 3/3 3/3 6/6 3/3
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table  2.  Analysis of variance (ANOVA) of nickel concentrations among species, gender and age
    Source of variation Mean square F p
    Species 78.23 8.56 0.01 a
    Gender 9.56 1.04 NS b
    Age 7.57 0.82 NS
    Intercept (species × gender × age) 572.55 62.66 0.001
    a p-value for 3-way ANOVA. Interaction term significant as indicated.
    b NS = not significant at p > 0.05.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Nickel is not an important trace element in organisms, but at high levels, they can cause adverse health effects. Sources of heavy metals vary considerably. It is emitted into the environment by both natural and man-made sources. Once released into the environment, nickel readily forms complexes with many ligands, making it more mobile than most heavy metals (Mansouri et al., 2011). Nickel is related to the pigmentation of feathers in birds and excreted via the feathers by moulting (Honda et al., 1986). Nickel concentrations in the current study (3.47–8.94 µg·g−1) were higher than those in Fulica atra (0.8 µg·g−1), Phalacrocorax carbo (0.5 µg·g−1) and Nycticorax nycticorax (1.2 µg·g−1) from Russia (Lebedeva, 1997), in Egretta alba (0.2 µg·g−1) from Korea (Honda et al., 1986) and Parus major (0.25 µg·g−1) from China (Deng et al., 2007). In our study, the nickel concentrations were similar to those in Bubulcus ibis L. (7.8–9.0 µg·g−1) from Pakistan (Malik and Zeb, 2009).

    Few studies have examined the effect of gender on the accumulation of heavy metals in feathers and other tissues (Burger, 1995; Zamani-Ahmadmahmoodi et al., 2010). Several studies reported no significant differences in the heavy metal content of feathers between male and female birds (Hutton, 1981; Zamani-Ahmadmahmoodi et al., 2009a). Similarly, in the present study there was no evidence of significant different accumulations between male and female birds (Table 2), suggesting that both sexes utilize similar foraging strategies in both species (Hindell et al., 1999). While studying heavy metals in the feathers of Larus dominicanus, Barbieri et al. (2009) showed that the levels of nickel concentration were higher in adults (5.92 µg·g−1) than in juveniles (2.23 µg·g−1). Similar levels of nickel have been detected in other seabird species from different parts of the world (Norheim, 1987). Adults have had several years to accumulate metals in their internal tissues; these can be mobilized into the blood and deposited in feathers during their formation (Burger, 1994). Elsewhere, Burger and Gochfeld (1991) pointed to heavy metal concentrations in feathers of adult birds that may reflect exposure obtained at other time of the year, including exposure at non-breeding areas. On the other hand, while they were studying heavy metal concentrations in the feathers of Herring Gulls (Larus argentatus) in Captree, Long Island, they showed that the cadmium concentration was higher in juveniles but the lead concentration was higher in adults (Burger, 1995). Differences in levels of metal concentrations in adults and fledglings might also occur if adults and young eat different types of food during the breeding season, or different-sized food items (Burger, 1996).

    Research has indicated that the concentration of heavy metals in the tissues of migratory birds is higher than that in resident birds (Pacyna et al., 2006; Zamani-Ahmadmahmoodi et al., 2009b). Siberian Gulls are winter visitors to Hara Biosphere Reserve, while Western Reef Heron are residents there. The results of the current study show that the amount of nickel in the Siberian Gull feathers is higher than in the Western Reef Heron.

    The results of three-way ANOVA showed there were significant differences between the Western Reef Heron and Siberian Gull (p < 0.01). The Siberian Gull showed higher nickel concentrations than the Western Reef Heron. Birds that are large fish eaters should accumulate higher levels than those that eat a range of different foods or smaller fish. Furthermore, levels of metal in birds should reflect the levels in the fish they consume (Burger, 2002). The main difference in nickel levels between Western Reef Heron and Siberian Gull could be the result of different phylogenetic origin and physiology (Teal, 1969; Welty, 1975; Deng et al. 2007), or because they grow their feathers in different breeding areas with different levels of background contamination. Also, metabolic rates vary inversely with body weight and directly with activities such as flight and rest. Being smaller than Western Reef Heron, Siberian Gull was expected to have a higher metabolic rate. Higher metabolic rates may cause fast accumulations of trace nickel in the Siberian Gull. In general, the Siberian Gull eats more invertebrates than the Western Reef Heron, catches some larger fish (between 20–25 cm in size), consumes offal discarded by fishing boats and eats dead fish found along the shore, while the Western Reef Heron eats smaller fish, amphibians and insects.

  • Ali S. 1931. The origin of mimicry in cuckoo eggs. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc, 34:1067-1070.
    Arcese P, Smith JNM, Hatch MI. 1996. Nest predation by cow-birds and its consequences for passerine demography. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 93:4608-4611.
    Avilés JM, Soler JJ, Prez-Contreras T. 2006. Dark nests and egg colour in birds: a possible functional role of ultraviolet reflectance in egg detectability. Proc R Soc Lond B, 273:2821-2829.
    Avilés JM, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Åsmul M, Møller AP. 2006. Rapid increase in cuckoo egg matching in a recently parasitized Reed Warbler population. J Evol Biol, 19:1901-1910.
    Avilés JM, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Møller AP. 2007. Envormental conditions influence egg color of Reed Warblers Acrocephalus scirpaceus and their parasite, the Common Cuckoo Cuculus canorus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 61:475-485.
    Baldamus E. 1892. Das Leben der euorpäischen Kuckucke. Parey, Berlin.
    Becking JH. 1981. Notes on the breeding of Indian cuckoos. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc, 78:201-231.
    Blyth E. 1835. Observations on the cuckoo. Mag Nat Hist, 8:325-340.
    Bogale BA, Kamata N, Mioko K, Sugita S. 2011. Quantity discrimination in Jungle Crows, Corvus macrorhynchos. Anim Behav, 82:635-641.
    Brewer TM. 1840. Wilson's American Ornithology. Otis, Broaders, Boston.
    Brooker MG, Brooker LC. 1989. The comparative breeding behaviour of two sympatric cuckoos, Horsefield's Bronze-cuckoo Chrysococcyx basalis and the Shining Bronze-cuckoo C. lucidus, in Western Australia: a new model for the evolution of egg morphology and host specificity in avian brood parasites. Ibis, 131:528-547.
    Brooker LC, Brooker MG. 1990. Why are cuckoos host specific?. Oikos, 57:301-309.
    Čapek V. 1896. Beiträge zur Fortpflanzungsgeschichte des Kuckucks. Ornithol Jahrbuch, 7:41-72, 102-118, 146-157, 165-183.
    Carpenter GDH. 1941-42. Observations and experiments in Africa by the late C. F. M. Swynnerton on wild birds eating butterflies and the preferences shown. Proc Linn Soc Lond, 154:10-34.
    Chance E[P]. 1922. The Cuckoo's Secret. Sidgwick and Jackson, London.
    Chance EP. 1940. The Truth about the Cuckoo. Country Life, London.
    Cherry MI, Bennett ATD. 2001. Egg colour matching in an African cuckoo, as revealed by ultraviolet-visible reflectance spectrophotometry. Proc R Soc Lond B, 268:565-571.
    Clark L, Mason JR. 1989. Sensitivity of Brown-headed Cowbirds to volatiles. Condor, 91:922-932.
    Cott HB. 1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
    Davies NB. 1999. Cuckoos and cowbirds versus hosts: co-evolutionary lag and equilibrium. Ostrich, 70:71-79.
    Davies NB. 2000. Cuckoos, Cowbirds and Other Cheats. Poyser, London.
    Davies NB, Brooke MdL. 1988. Cuckoos versus Reed Warblers: adaptations and counteradaptations. Anim Behav, 36:262-284. Davies NB, Brooke MdL. 1989. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. I. Host egg discrimination. J Anim Ecol, 58:207-224.
    Davies NB, Brooke MdL. 1998. Cuckoos versus hosts: experimental evidence for co-evolution. In: Rothstein SI, Robinson SK (eds) Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts: Studies in Coevolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 59-79.
    Dawkins R, Krebs JR. 1979. Arms races between and within species. Proc R Soc Lond B, 205:489-511.
    Dufty AM Jr. 1983. Variation in the egg markings of the Brown-headed Cowbird. Condor, 85:109-111.
    Friedmann H. 1964. The history of our knowledge of avian brood parasitism. Centaurus, 10:282-304.
    Gärtner K. 1981. Das Wegnehmen von Wirtsvogeleiern durch den Kuckuck (Cuculus canorus). Ornithol Mitt, 33:15-131.
    Gehringer F. 1979. Etude sur le pillage par le Coucou, Cuculus canorus, des œufs de la Rousserolle effarvatte. Nos Oiseaux, 35:1-16.
    Gesner C. 1669. Vogelbuch. Wilhelm Serlins, Frankfurt-am-Main.
    Gibbs HL, Sorenson MD, Marchetti K, Brooke MdL, Davies NB, Nakamura H. 2000. Genetic evidence for female host-specific races of the Common Cuckoo. Nature, 407:83-186.
    Gill FB, Wright M. 2006. Birds of the World. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA.
    Gurney JH. 1899. The economy of the cuckoo (Cuculus canorus). Trans Norfolk Norwich Nat Hist Soc, 6:365-384.
    Hagelin JC, Jones IL. 2007. Bird odors and other chemical substances: a defense mechanism or overlooked mode of intraspecific communication? Auk, 124:741-761.
    Hamilton WJ, Ⅲ, Orians GH. 1965. Evolution of brood parasitism in altricial birds. Condor, 37:361-382.
    Hann HW. 1937. Life history of the oven-bird in southern Michigan. Wilson Bull, 49:145-237.
    Higuchi H. 1998. Host use and egg color of Japanese cuckoos. In: Rothstein SI, Robinson SK (eds) Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts: Studies in Coevolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 80-93.
    Honza M, Požgayová M, Procházka P, Tkadlec E. 2007. Consistency in egg rejection behaviour: responses to repeated brood parasitism in the Blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla). Ethology, 113:344-351.
    Honza M, Taborsky B, Taborsky M, Teuschl Y, Vogl W, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. 2002. Behaviour of female Common Cuckoos, Cuculus canorus, in the vicinity of host nests before and during laying: a radiotelemetry study. Anim Behav, 64:861-868.
    Hoover JP. 2003. Multiple effects of brood parasitism reduce the reproductive success of Prothonotary Warblers, Protonaria citrea. Anim Behav, 65:923-935.
    Hoover JP, Robinson SK. 2007. Retaliatory mafia behavior by a parasitic cowbird favors host acceptance of parasitic eggs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 104:4474-4483. .
    Jenner E. 1788. Observations on the natural history of the cuckoo. Phil Trans R Soc Lond, 78:219-237.
    Jourdain FCR. 1925. A study on parasitism in the cuckoos. Proc Zool Soc Lond, 1925:639-667.
    Junker T. 2003. Ornithology and the genesis of the synthetic theory of evolution. Avian Sci, 3:65-73.
    Kelly C. 1987. A model to explore the rate of spread of mimicry and rejection in hypothetical populations of cuckoos and their hosts. J Theor Biol, 125:282-299.
    Kilner RM. 2006. The evolution of egg colour and patterning in birds. Biol Rev, 81:383-406.
    Kim CH, Yamagishi S, Won PO. 1995. Egg-color dimorphism and breeding success of the Crow Tit (Paradoxornis webbianus). Auk, 112:831-839.
    Kim DW. 2006. Egg discrimination ability of Paradoxornis webbianus and antiparasitic behavior against brood parasitism. MSc thesis, Kyunghee University, Seoul, Korea.
    Lahti DC, Lahti AR. 2002. How precise is egg discrimination in weaverbirds? Anim Behav, 63:1135-1142.
    Langmore NE, Stevens M, Maurer G, Kilner RM. 2009. Are dark cuckoo eggs cryptic in host nests? Anim Behav, 78:461-468.
    Lee JW, Kim DW, Yoo JC. 2005. Egg rejection by both male and female Vinous-throated Parrotbill Paradoxornis webbianus. Integr Biosci, 9:211-213.
    Lee Y. 2008. Egg discrimination by the vinous-throated parrotbill, a host of the common cuckoo that lays polychromatic eggs. MSc thesis, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada.
    Leverkühn P. 1891. Fremde Eier im Nest: ein Beitrag zur Biologie der Vögel. Friedländer und Sohn, Berlin.
    Liang W, Yang C, Antonov A, Fossøy F, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Shykoff JA, Møller AP, Takasu F. 2011. Sex roles in egg recognition and egg polymorphism in avian brood parasitism. Behav Ecol, 23:397-402.
    Lindholm AK. 1997. Evolution of host defences against avian brood parasitism. PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom.
    Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. 1991. Rejection of cuckoo eggs in relation to host age: a possible evolutionary equilibrium. Behav Ecol, 3:128-132.
    Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. 1995. Constraints on egg discrimination and cuckoo-host co-evolution. Anim Behav, 49:1185-1209.
    Lottinger AJ. 1775. Le coucou. Discours apologétique, ou mé-moire sur le coucou d'Europe. JB Hiacinthe LeClerc, Nancy, France.
    Lottinger AJ. 1795. Histoire du coucou d'Europe. FG Levrault, Strasbourg, France.
    Lucas AHS. 1887. On the production of colour in birds' eggs. Trans Proc R Soc Victoria, 24:52-60.
    Lyon BE. 2003. Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism. Nature, 422:495-499.
    McMaster DG, Neudorf, DLH, Sealy SG, Pitcher TE. 2004. A comparative analysis of laying times in passerine birds. J Field Ornithol, 75:113-122.
    Marshall GAK. 1938. [Obituary of] Mr. C.F.M. Swynnerton, C.M.G. Nature, 142:198-199.
    Mayfield H. 1960. The Kirtland's Warbler. Cranbrook Institute of Science, Bloomfield Hills, Michigan.
    Mayfield H. 1965. Chance distribution of cowbird eggs. Condor, 67:257-263.
    Mayr E, Provine wb (eds). 1980. The Evolutionary Synthesis: Perspectives on the Unification of Biology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
    Martínez JG, Soler JJ, Soler M, Burke T. 1998. Spatial patterns of egg laying and multiple parasitism in a brood parasite: a non-territorial system in the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius). Oecologia, 117:286-294.
    Mason P, Rothstein SI. 1986. Coevolution and avian brood parasitism: cowbird eggs show evolutionary response to host discrimination. Evolution, 40:1207-1214.
    McEwan AJ, Joy MK. 2011. Monitoring a New Zealand freshwater fish community using passive integrated transponder (PIT) technology; lessons learned and recommendations for future use. NZ Mar Freshwater Res, 45:121-133.
    McLaren CM, Sealy SG. 2000. Are nest predation and brood parasitism correlated in Yellow Warblers? A test of the cowbird predation hypothesis. Auk, 117:1056-1060.
    McLaren CM, Woolfenden BE, Gibbs HL, Sealy SG. 2003. Genetic and temporal patterns of multiple parasitism by Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater) on Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia). Can J Zool, 81:1-6.
    Mermoz ME, Reboreda JC. 1999. Egg-laying behaviour by Shiny Cowbirds parasitizing Brown-and-yellow Marshbirds. Anim Behav, 58:873-882.
    Moksnes A. 1992. Egg recognition in Chaffinches and Bramblings. Anim Behav, 44:993-995.
    Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Braa AT. 1991. Rejection behavior by Common Cuckoo hosts towards artificial brood parasite eggs. Auk, 108:348-354.
    Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Solli MM. 1994. Documenting puncture ejection of parasitic eggs by Chaffinches Fringilla coelebs and Blackcaps Sylvia atricapilla. Fauna norv. Series C, Cinclus, 17:115-118.
    Moreno J, Osorno JL. 2003. Avian egg colour and sexual selection: does eggshell pigmentation reflect female condition and genetic quality? Ecol Lett, 6:803-806.
    Moskát C, Bán M, Székley T, Komdeur J, Lucassen RWG, van Boheemen LA, Hauber ME. 2010. Discordancy or template-based recognition? Dissecting the cognitive basis of the rejection of foreign eggs in hosts of avian brood parasites. J Exp Biol, 213:1976-1983.
    Moskát C, Honza, M. 2002. European Cuckoo Cuculus canorus parasitism and host's rejection behaviour in a heavily parasitized Great Reed Warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus population. Ibis, 144:614-622.
    Nakamura T, Cruz A. 2000. The ecology of egg-puncture behavior in the Shiny Cowbird in southwestern Puerto Rico. In: Smith JNM, Cook TL, Rothstein SI, Robinson SK, Sealy SG (eds) Ecology and Management of Cowbirds and Their Hosts. University of Texas Press, Austin, pp 178-186.
    Nakamura H, Miyazawa Y. 1997. Movements, space use and social organization of radio-tracked Common Cuckoos during the breeding season in Japan. Jap J Ornithol, 46:23-54.
    Newton A. 1869. Cuckows' eggs. Nature, 1:74-76.
    Nolan V Jr. 1978. The ecology and behavior of the Prairie Warbler Dendroica discolor. Ornithol Monogr, No. 26.
    Orians GH, Røskaft E, Beletsky LD. 1989. Do Brown-headed Cowbirds lay their eggs at random in the nests of Red-winged Blackbirds. Wilson Bull, 101:599-605.
    Ortega CP, Ortega JC, Cruz A. 1994. Use of artificial Brown-headed Cowbird eggs as a potential management tool in deterring parasitism. J Wildl Manage, 58:488-492.
    Pagel M, Møller AP, Pomiankowski A. 1998. Reduced parasitism by retaliatory cuckoos selects for hosts that rear cuckoo nestlings. Behav Ecol, 9:566-572.
    Payne RB. 1977. The ecology of brood parasitism in birds. Annu Rev Ecol Syst, 8:1-28.
    Palomino JJ, Martin-Vivaldi M, Soler M, Soler JJ. 1998. Females are responsible for ejection of cuckoo eggs in the Rufous Bush Robin. Anim Behav, 56:131-136.
    Peer BD, Sealy SG. 2001. Mechanism of egg recognition in the Great-tailed Grackle (Quiscalus mexicanus). Bird Behav, 14:71-73.
    Peer BD, Sealy SG. 2004. Fate of grackle (Quiscalus spp. ) defenses in the absence of brood parasitism: implications for long-term parasite-host coevolution. Auk, 121:1172-1186.
    Peer BD, Rothstein SI, Delaney KS, Fleischer RC. 2007. Defence behaviour against brood parasitism is deeply rooted in mainland and island scrub-jays. Anim Behav, 73:55-63.
    Pepperberg IM. 2001. Avian cognitive abilities. Bird Behav, 14:51-70.
    Picman J. 1989. Mechanism of puncture resistance of eggs of Brown-headed Cowbirds. Auk, 106:577-583.
    Poulton EB. 1890. The colours of animals: their meaning and use, especially considered in the case of insects. Int Sci Ser, no. 68.
    Poulsen H. 1953. A study of incubation responses and some other behavior patterns in birds. Vidensk Medd fra Dansk Naturh Foren, 115:1-131.
    Požgayová M, Procházka P, Honza M. 2009. Sex-specific defence behavior against brood parasitism in a host with female-only incubation. Behav Processes, 81:34-38.
    Preston FW. 1948. The cowbird (M. ater) and the cuckoo (C. canorus). Ecology, 29:115-116.
    Rasmussen JL, Sealy SG, Underwood TJ. 2009. Video recording reveals the method of ejection of Brown headed Cowbird eggs and no cost in American Robins and Gray Catbirds. Condor, 111:570-574.
    Reboreda JC, Clayton NS, Kacelnik A. 1996. Species and sex differences in hippocampus size in parasitic and non-parasitic cowbirds. Neuroreport, 7:505-508.
    Rennie J. 1831. The Architecture of Birds. Charles Knight, Lon-don.
    Rensch B. 1924. Zur Entstehung der Mimikry der Kuckuckseier. J Ornithol, 72:461-472.
    Rensch B. 1925. Verhalten von Singvögeln bei Aenderung des Geleges. Ornithol Monatschr, 33:169-173.
    Rey E. 1892. Altes und Neues aus dem Haushalte des Kuckucks. Freese, Leipzig.
    Rivers JW, Young S, Gonzalez EG, Horton B, Lock J, Fleischer RC. 2012. High levels of relatedness between Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater) nestmates in a heavily parasitized host community. Auk, 129:623-631.
    Roper TJ. 1999. Olfaction in birds. Adv Stud Behav, 28:247-332.
    Rothstein SI. 1970. An experimental investigation of the defenses of the hosts of the parasitic Brown-headed Cowbird (Molo-thrus ater). PhD dissertation, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut.
    Rothstein SI. 1974. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: possible learned and innate factors. Auk, 91:796-807.
    Rothstein SI. 1975a. An experimental and teleonomic investigation of avian brood parasitism. Condor, 77:250-271.
    Rothstein SI. 1975b. Mechanisms of avian egg-recognition: do birds know their own eggs? Anim Behav, 23:268-278.
    Rothstein SI. 1975c. Evolutionary rates and host defenses against avian brood parasitism. Am Nat, 109:161-176.
    Rothstein SI. 1977. Cowbird parasitism and egg recognition of the Northern Oriole. Wilson Bull, 89:21-32.
    Rothstein SI. 1978. Mechanisms of avian egg-recognition: additional evidence for learned components. Anim Behav, 26:671-677.
    Rothstein SI. 1982a. Successes and failures in avian egg and nestling recognition with comments on the utility of optimality reasoning. Am Zool, 22:547-560.
    Rothstein SI. 1982b. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: which parameters elicit responses by ejecter species? Behav Ecol Soiciobiol, 11:229-239.
    Rothstein SI. 1990. A model system for coevolution: avian brood parasitism. Annu Rev Ecol Syst, 21:481-508.
    Rothstein SI, Robinson SK. 1998. The evolution and ecology of avian brood parasitism. In: Rothstein SI, Robinson SK (eds) Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts: Studies in Coevolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 3-56.
    Schmaltz G, Somers CM, Sharma P, Quinn JS. 2006. Non-destructive sampling of maternal DNA from the external shell of bird eggs. Conserv Genet, 7:543-549.
    Schulze-Hagen K, Stokke BG, Birkhead TR. 2009. Reproductive biology of the European Cuckoo Cuculus canorus: early insights, persistent errors and the acquisition of knowledge. J Ornithol, 150:1-16.
    Sealy SG. 1992. Removal of Yellow Warbler eggs in association with cowbird parasitism. Condor, 94:40-54.
    Sealy SG. 1996. Evolution of host defenses against brood parasitism: implications for puncture-ejection by a small passerine. Auk, 113:346-355.
    Sealy SG. 2009. Cuckoos and their fosterers: uncovering details of Edward Blyth's field experiments. Arch Nat Hist, 36:129-135.
    Sealy SG, Bazin RC. 1995. Low frequency of observed cowbird parasitism on Eastern Kingbirds: host rejection, effective nest defense, or parasite avoidance? Behav Ecol, 6:140-145.
    Sealy SG, Guigueno MF. 2011. Cuckoo chicks evicting their nest mates: coincidental observations by Edward Jenner in England and Antoine Joseph Lottinger in France. Arch Nat Hist, 38:220-228.
    Sealy SG, Lorenzana JC. 1998. Yellow Warblers (Dendroica petechia) do not recognize their own eggs. Bird Behav, 12:57-66.
    Sealy SG, Neudorf DL. 1995. Male Northern Orioles eject cow-bird eggs: Implications for the evolution of rejection behavior. Condor, 369-375.
    Sealy SG, McMaster DG, Peer BD. 2002. Tactics of obligate brood parasites to secure suitable incubators. In: Deeming DC (ed) Avian Incubation: Behaviour, Environment, and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 254-269.
    Sealy SG, Neudorf DL, Hill DP. 1995. Rapid laying by Brown-headed Cowbirds and other parasitic birds. Ibis, 137:76-84.
    Sherry DF, Vaccarino AL, Buckenham K, Herz RS. 1989. The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds. Brain Behav Evol, 34:308-317.
    Sherry DF, Forbes ML, Khurgel M, Ivy GO. 1993. Females have a larger hippocampus than males in the brood-parasitic Brown-headed Cowbird. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 90:7839-7843.
    Smith JNM, Taitt MJ, Zanette L, Myers-Smith IH. 2003. How do Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater) cause nest failure in Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia)? A removal experiment. Auk, 120:772-783.
    Soler JJ, Møller AP, Soler M. 1998. Mafia behavior and the evolution of facultative virulence. J Theor Biol, 191:267-277.
    Soler JJ, Sorci G, Soler M, Møller AP. 1999. Change in host rejection behavior mediated by the predatory behavior of its brood parasite. Behav Ecol, 10:275-180.
    Soler M, Soler JJ, Martínez JG. 1998. Duration of sympatry and coevolution between the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamater glandarius) and its primary host, the Magpie (Pica pica). In: Rothstein SI, Robinson SK (eds) Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts: Studies in Coevolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 113-142.
    Soler M, Fernández-Morante, Espinosa F, Martín-Vivaldi M. 2012. Pecking but accepting the parasitic eggs may not reflect ejection failure: the role of motivation. Ethology, 118:662-672.
    Soler M, Martín-Vivaldi M, Pérez-Contreras T. 2002. Identifi-cation of the sex that is responsible for recognition and the method of ejection of parasite eggs in some potential Common Cuckoo hosts. Ethology, 108:1093-1101.
    Soler M, Soler JJ, Martínez JG. 1997. Great Spotted Cuckoos improve their reproductive success by damaging magpie host eggs. Anim Behav, 54:1227-1233.
    Soler M, Soler JJ, Martínez JG, Møller AP. 1995. Magpie host manipulation by Great Spotted Cuckoos: evidence for an avian mafia? Evolution, 49:770-775.
    Strausberger BM, Rothstein SI. 2009. Parasitic cowbirds may defeat defense by causing rejecters to misimprint of cowbird eggs. Behav Ecol, 20:691-699.
    Stuart Baker EC. 1913. The evolution of adaptation in parasitic cuckoos' eggs. Ibis, Series 10, 1:384-398.
    Stuart Baker EC. 1942. Cuckoo Problems. Witherby, London.
    Swynnerton CFM. 1916. On the coloration of the mouths and eggs of birds.-Ⅱ. On the coloration of eggs. Ibis, Series 10, 4:529-606.
    Swynnerton CFM. 1918. Rejections by birds of eggs unlike their own: with remarks on some of the cuckoo problems. Ibis, Series 10, 6:127-154.
    Underwood TJ, Sealy SG. 2002. Adaptive significance of egg coloration. In: Deeming DC (ed) Avian Incubation: Behaviour, Environment, and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 280-298.
    Underwood TJ, Sealy SG. 2006. Parameters of Brown-headed Cowbird Molothrus ater egg recognition and ejection in War-bling Vireos Vireo gilvus. J Avian Biol, 37:457-466.
    Underwood TJ, Sealy SG. 2008. UV reflectance of eggs of Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater) and acceptor and rejecter hosts. J Ornithol, 149:313-321.
    Underwood TJ, Sealy SG. 2011. Behavior of Warbling Vireos ejecting real and artificial cowbird eggs. Wilson J Ornithol, 123:395-400.
    Underwood TJ, Sealy SG, McLaren CM. 2004. Experiments on egg discrimination in two North American corvids: further evidence for retention of egg rejection. Can J Zool, 82:1399-1407.
    Victoria JK. 1972: Clutch characteristics and egg discriminative ability of the African Village Weaverbird Ploceus cucullatus. Ibis, 114:367-376.
    Weatherhead PJ. 1989. Sex ratios, host-specific reproductive success, and impact of Brown-headed Cowbird. Auk, 106:358-366.
    Welty JC. 1962. The life of birds. WB Saunders, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
    Whittaker DJD, Reichard DG, Dapper AL, Ketterson ED. 2009. Behavioral responses of nesting female Dark-eyed Juncos Junco hyemalis to hetero-and conspecific passerine preen oils. J Avian Biol, 40:579-583.
    Wilson A. 1810. American Ornithology. Vol. 2. Bradford and In-skeep, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA.
    Wuketits FM. 2006. Bernhard Rensch, German evolutionist. Biol Theory, 1:410-413.
    Wyllie I. 1981. The Cuckoo. Universe, New York.
    Yang C, Cai Y, Liang W. 2012. Species identification of sympatric cuckoo nestlings in a multiple-cuckoo system, China. Chinese Birds, 3:108-112.
    Zahavi A. 1979. Parasitism and nest predation in parasitic cuckoos. Am Nat, 113:157-159.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(4)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (4148) PDF downloads (1974) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return