George P. Tiley, Akanksha Pandey, Rebecca T. Kimball, Edward L. Braun, J. Gordon Burleigh. 2020: Whole genome phylogeny of Gallus: introgression and data-type effects. Avian Research, 11(1): 7. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-020-00194-w
Citation: George P. Tiley, Akanksha Pandey, Rebecca T. Kimball, Edward L. Braun, J. Gordon Burleigh. 2020: Whole genome phylogeny of Gallus: introgression and data-type effects. Avian Research, 11(1): 7. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-020-00194-w

Whole genome phylogeny of Gallus: introgression and data-type effects

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    J. Gordon Burleigh, gburleigh@ufl.edu

  • Received Date: 05 Nov 2019
  • Accepted Date: 06 Mar 2020
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 16 Mar 2020
  • Background 

    Previous phylogenetic studies that include the four recognized species of Gallus have resulted in a number of distinct topologies, with little agreement. Several factors could lead to the failure to converge on a consistent topology, including introgression, incomplete lineage sorting, different data types, or insufficient data.

    Methods 

    We generated three novel whole genome assemblies for Gallus species, which we combined with data from the published genomes of Gallus gallus and Bambusicola thoracicus (a member of the sister genus to Gallus). To determine why previous studies have failed to converge on a single topology, we extracted large numbers of orthologous exons, introns, ultra-conserved elements, and conserved non-exonic elements from the genome assemblies. This provided more than 32 million base pairs of data that we used for concatenated maximum likelihood and multispecies coalescent analyses of Gallus.

    Results 

    All of our analyses, regardless of data type, yielded a single, well-supported topology. We found some evidence for ancient introgression involving specific Gallus lineages as well as modest data type effects that had an impact on support and branch length estimates in specific analyses. However, the estimated gene tree spectra for all data types had a relatively good fit to their expectation given the multispecies coalescent.

    Conclusions 

    Overall, our data suggest that conflicts among previous studies probably reflect the use of smaller datasets (both in terms of number of sites and of loci) in those analyses. Our results demonstrate the importance of sampling large numbers of loci, each of which has a sufficient number of sites to provide robust estimates of gene trees. Low-coverage whole genome sequencing, as we did here, represents a cost-effective means to generate the very large data sets that include multiple data types that enabled us to obtain a robust estimate of Gallus phylogeny.

  • In birds, coloration is one of the most important traits linked to social status, physiological state and sexual behavior (Hill and McGraw 2006). Parrots inhabiting different regions of the world constitute a group that has received special attention largely because of their colorful displays (del Hoyo et al. 1992). The wide variation in coloration observed in parrots is mainly the product of the nano-structure of their feathers (structural coloration) plus the presence of different kinds of pigments (melanins and a unique kind of pigment called psittacofulvins) (Stradi et al. 2001; McGraw and Nogare 2004; Berg and Bennett 2010). Furthermore, like all diurnal avian species, parrots perceive plumage coloration differently from mammals thanks to their tetrachromatic visual system, which includes a UV photoreceptor (Vorobyev et al. 1998). Because of this difference in the avian visual system, studies of plumage coloration relating to intra-specific signaling should assess coloration in a way that takes into account how color is perceived by other birds, especially in those species classified as sexually monochromatic according to the human visual system (del Hoyo et al. 1992). This is important since for a long time it was thought that sexual differences in plumage coloration and size tended to be weaker in monogamous than in polygamous species (Andersson 1994). Owens and Hartley (1998) demonstrated for the first time that size dimorphism and plumage color dimorphism in birds do not vary in the same way. They concluded that sexual differences in size are associated with the kind of mating system characteristic of the species and with sexual differences in parental care, that is, greater sexual differences in size at higher levels of polygamy and less parental care of the male. In terms of sexual differences in plumage coloration, they determined these to be associated with high levels of frequency of extra bond paternity exhibited by the bird species (Owens and Hartley 1998). Added to this, it has been reported that in the Psittacidae family, two South American species (Blue-fronted Amazon Amazona aestiva and the Burrowing Parrot Cyanoliseus patagonus) which are considered monogamous and without sexual size dimorphism, exhibit differences between sexes that are visually indistinguishable by humans (Santos et al. 2006; Masello et al. 2009). Although providing information on only two South American species, these studies suggest that incorporating the UV region into the discrimination analysis may reveal that sexual plumage color dimorphism could be more frequent than previously thought (Hausmann et al. 2003; Santos et al. 2006; Masello et al. 2009).

    In this context, the Monk Parakeet (Myiopsitta monachus), another Psittacidae family member native to South America, which has hitherto been considered to be sexually monomorphic, presents an interesting case for study of plumage color and size dimorphism (del Hoyo et al. 1992). The Monk Parakeet is a colonial parrot considered to be socially monogamous; however, recent genetic studies provide evidence of extra-pair paternity, breeding trios and cases of intra-brood parasitism (Martínez et al. 2013; Bucher et al. 2016). During summer, the Monk Parakeet uses sticks to build nests with multiple individual chambers occupied by different pairs and to a lesser extent by trios (del Hoyo et al. 1992; Spreyer et al. 1998; Forshaw 2010; Hobson et al. 2014, 2015).

    Like other parrots, the Monk Parakeet has a combination of structural and pigmentary coloration: to the human eye it looks green over most of its body, gray on its head and belly, and blue on the flight feathers. No studies have quantified plumage color dimorphism and little regarding to size dimorphism (Martinez et al. 2018) in this parrot. Based on the background data above, we aimed to determine whether adult Monk Parakeet males and females exhibit sexual plumage color dimorphism and size dimorphism in any of their body regions. To this end, we used spectrophotometry to objectively measure plumage reflectance across the visible spectrum of birds (300–700 nm) and performed morphometric analysis.

    Thirty-two adult wild male and thirty-six adult wild female Monk parakeets (Psittacidae family, Psittacinae subfamily, tribe Arini) were captured inside Córdoba Zoo, Argentina (31° 25′ 31.79″ S, 64° 10′ 29.92″ W) with passive traps, following the procedure described in Valdez and Benitez-Vieyra (2016) to trap doves. Captures were made during May and June 2017, thus avoiding the molting period in the Southern hemisphere (December to April) (Navarro et al. 1992). All animals were sacrificed using sodium pentobarbital and sexed by examination of the reproductive organs; the skins were then used in the spectrophotometry measurements. We determined and compared the reflectance spectrum of twelve different body regions: forehead, crown, cheeks, nape, back, chest, belly, blue wing coverts, green wing coverts, primary and secondary remiges, and tail (on all upper surfaces). The animals were sacrificed as part of a neuroendocrine study.

    Bird coloration cannot be accurately analyzed with tools designed for human vision, as birds perceive colors in a radically different way. Thus, we carried out all reflectance measurements within the avian spectral sensitivity range (300–700 nm, Bowmaker et al. 1997) using an Ocean Optics USB4000 spectrophotometer equipped with a halogen and a deuterium light source (830 Douglas Ave., Dunedin, FL, USA 34698), both connected to the sensor by a bifurcated fiber optic cable. Each plumage region was illuminated, and the light reflected at 45° was collected. The distance between the probe and the plumage was 4 mm, the spectrophotometer resolution 0.19 nm, the integration time 300 ms and each spectrum was the average of three readings. A white standard (Ocean Optics, WS-1-SS White Standard) was used to re-calibrate the equipment between measurements in order to correct for possible shifts in performance. Reflectance was measured using SpectraSuite software (Ocean Optics, Inc.).

    After obtaining reflectance spectra, we applied a receptor-noise limited model of avian vision (Vorobyev et al. 1998) to estimate how avian receivers of chromatic signals would perceive the parakeets' plumage colorations. This model takes into account the number and sensitivity of color receptors in the avian eye (cones) and how color information is processed in terms of signal-to-noise ratio, assuming that color discrimination is limited by photoreceptor noise. To apply an avian visual model to reflectance data, we used the pavo 2.4.0 package (Maia et al. 2013) for R (Team RC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing 2013). Cone quantum catch (Q) for each of the four avian cones was calculated under standardized daylight illumination (D65) as a representative spectrum for open habitat ambient light at midday, similar to the type of habitat that this species frequents. We used UV-type avian eyes for spectral cone sensitivities as a general representative of the parrot visual system (Bowmaker et al. 1997). Although cone parameters have not been measured in M. monachus, we used the generalized spectral cone sensitivities of the UV-type of Melopsittacus undulatus eyes since this is the only member of the Psittaciformes that has been characterized to date (Bowmaker et al. 1997; Goldsmith and Butler 2005; Lind et al. 2014). Contrasts between males–males, males‒females and females–females were characterized in units of "just noticeable differences" (JND), such that one JND represents the threshold of possible discrimination. Chromatic (dS) and achromatic (dL) distances were calculated in JNDs following the vision model using cues. Visual stimuli separated by one JND are discernible by birds, although only under ideal illumination (Olsson et al. 2015).

    For each Monk Parakeet we measured the height, width and length of the bill (from the tip to the base of the skull), length of the tarsus, total length and wing length. For this we used a digital caliper (range 0‒150 mm; resolution 0.01 mm; accuracy ± 0.02 mm) and a millimeter metal ruler (50 cm). The animals were also weighed with a PESOLA brand spring scale (accuracy ± 2 g).

    First, we examined the mean ± 2SE reflectance spectra of males and females to determine the presence of overlapping regions. Then, we calculated all pairwise chromatic and achromatic distances (measured in JNDs) among males, among females, and between males and females, for each body region. We tested whether between-sex differences were greater than within-sex differences using a permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA, Anderson 2001), as implemented in the adonis function of the vegan R package (Oksanen 2017). PERMANOVA is a multivariate analogue of the univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) where multivariate variance is partitioned in the space of any arbitrary dissimilarity measure, JNDs in our case. In this test, significance is obtained by comparing the multivariate version of the F-statistic with the results obtained under a large number of permutations. We performed 24 PERMANOVAs (12 for achromatic distances and 12 for chromatic distances) to test for differences between sexes in each body part. Each test involved 999 permutations. We considered the observed distance between sexes to be significantly different from that expected by random chance if it was greater than 95% of the randomized values.

    Finally, we examined the multivariate morphological variation between sexes in the six traits detailed above by principal component analysis (PCA). In addition, we applied a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to test for multivariate differences between sexes. In contrast with color cues, differences in morphology can be expressed in terms of euclidean distances among individuals, fulfilling the assumptions of common (parametric) MANOVA. When a significant effect was detected, we performed additional univariate nested ANOVAs to determine which traits accounted for the significant effect in the MANOVA.

    No noticeable differences between sexes were observed in spectral shape for any of the studied body regions. There was considerable overlapping of mean reflectance values in some regions. The comparison between the mean ± 2SE reflectance spectra of males and females for the twelve different body regions examined throughout the avian visual range (300‒700 nm) is shown in Fig. 1b.

    Figure  1.  Spectrophotometric analysis of Monk Parakeet plumage. a Photograph of the Monk Parakeet in its colonial nest. b Reflectance spectra (300‒700 nm) of the twelve Monk Parakeet body regions. Each spectrum represents the mean reflectance ± 2SE of 32 males (blue) and 36 females (red)

    The achromatic and chromatic distances in JNDs among males, among females and between males and females are shown in Fig. 2. In all body regions examined, mean achromatic distances were higher than 1 JND (discrimination limit under ideal illumination). These indicate that on average, individuals could be distinguished based on achromatic cues, even when they are of the same sex (Fig. 2). Nevertheless, mean achromatic distances were not significantly different between sexes (PERMANOVA, in all cases F1, 67 < 4.117 and p > 0.06), with the exception of the blue wing coverts (F1, 67 = 5.512, p = 0.021) and nape (F1, 67 = 4.524, p = 0.042).

    Figure  2.  Achromatic and chromatic distances after applying an avian visual model. Mean values ± standard deviations of achromatic and chromatic distances measured as just noticeable differences (JNDs) following a receptor-noise limited model of avian color vision. Vertical dotted lines indicate the discrimination threshold of one JND. At values below 1, individual chromatic and achromatic cues cannot be distinguished. Permutational multivariate analyses of variance (PERMANOVAs) were used to test whether or not differences between sexes were greater than within sexes. Significant differences (p < 0.05) are indicated with an asterisk (*)

    In the mean chromatic distances, we found that three (forehead, cheeks and belly) of the twelve body regions studied were below the discrimination limit 1, indicating that individuals cannot be distinguished based on these cues. In the other nine corporal regions (crown, nape, back, chest, blue wing coverts, green wing coverts, primary remiges, secondary remiges and tail) mean chromatic distances were equal or higher than the discrimination limit. In seven corporal regions (nape, back, chest, green wing coverts, primary remiges, secondary remiges and tail) no significant differences between males and females were observed (PERMANOVA, in all cases F1, 67 < 3.076, p > 0.090) (Fig. 2). The only two exceptions were the blue wing coverts (PERMANOVA, F1, 67 = 4.28, p = 0.025) and the crown (F1, 67 = 5.659, p = 0.013). However, notice that the average color distance between sexes for the crown was only 1.109 JNDs (Fig. 2), suggesting that this difference between sexes is hardly noticeable for birds.

    The first three principal components of the PCA explained 72.31% of the total variation recorded among individuals (Fig. 3). All traits had positive, similar loadings on PC1, suggesting that this component is associated with general differences in size among individuals (Table 1). Bill traits and total weight attained positive loadings on PC2, while wing length, tarsus length and total length attained negative loadings (Table 1). Thus, positive PC2 scores correspond to heavier birds with more prominent bills, but with shorter wings, tarsus and total length. The opposite is true for individuals with negative PC2 scores. Finally, total length had a strong positive loading on PC3, while bill width attained a strong and negative score (Table 1), indicating that this PC expresses differences among individuals in these traits. MANOVA indicated that there were significant differences between sexes (Wilks' λ = 0.554, p < 0.0001) and univariate ANOVAs showed these differences to be accounted for by body weight (F1, 66 = 18.524, p = 0.0001), bill height (F1, 66 = 27.118, p < 0.0001), bill length (F1, 66 = 13.471, p = 0.0005) and bill width (F1, 66 = 25.183, p < 0.0001), but not by tarsus, wing and total length (in all cases F1, 66 < 2.038, p > 0.158; Fig. 3).

    Figure  3.  Morphometric analysis of the Monk Parakeet. Morphological differences between sexes in Monk Parakeet. a PCA for the five variables analyzed (height, width and length of the bill; length of the tarsus; total length, total weight and wing length). Polygons indicate convex hulls, and females are in orange and males in blue. b Box and whisker plots of those morphometric variables for which significant differences were detected using one-way ANOVAs. Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles (lower and upper quartiles, respectively) of the distribution, while the central band indicates the median. "Whiskers" indicate 1.5 times the interquartile range. Points indicate outliers
    Table  1.  Variable loadings and proportion of the variance explained by the first three principal components
    Variable PC1 PC2 PC3
    Bill length 0.428 0.108 0.398
    Bill width 0.385 0.402 < 0.001
    Bill height 0.429 0.265 ‒ 0.508
    Total weight 0.336 0.432 0.163
    Wing length 0.380 ‒ 0.444 ‒ 0.165
    Total length 0.338 ‒ 0.435 0.578
    Tarsus length 0.338 ‒ 0.428 ‒ 0.442
    Proportion of the variance explained 0.395 0.218 0.110
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In this work we examined the sexual plumage color dimorphism and size dimorphism in the colonial breeder Monk Parakeet (M. monachus) using an objective methodology (spectrophotometry) and morphometric analysis. The superposition of the reflectance spectra shows that overall, mean reflectance values are the same for both sexes in all body regions studied (see Fig. 1b). On the other hand, the achromatic and chromatic distances obtained with the avian visual model indicate a subtle sexual plumage color dimorphism in this parrot. Analysis of the mean pairwise achromatic distances showed all body regions to be above the discrimination limit for both sexes, with only slight differences between males and females for only blue wing coverts and nape. These findings may indicate that males and females are able to differentiate among individuals but not discern their sex based on plumage brightness. An interesting result regarding the parakeets' blue wing coverts is that they have mean achromatic distances above the discrimination level along with significant differences between the sexes. These feathers are only visible when the birds are flying, and are hidden when they are resting. This leads us to the question of the role these feathers play in sex differentiation: could they be involved in sex recognition during flight? Are they involved in mate choice? More detailed studies are necessary to answer these questions. Furthermore, the differences between male and female Monk Parakeets' nape achromatic distances are subtle (see Fig. 2), with values close to the discrimination limit, making it difficult to gauge the biological significance of the findings. Regarding the chromatic distances, three body regions showed chromatic distance values below the discrimination threshold, indicating that it is unlikely that individuals could be distinguished based on the coloration of these regions. The nine remaining body regions had distance values equal to or higher than the discrimination limit and seven of them did not show significant differences between males and females. The blue wing coverts and the crown were the only body regions observed to differ between males and females. The blue wing coverts were the only body region showing significant differences in both distances, but again, these feathers are hidden at rest and are only visible during flight.

    The crown on the other hand is of particular interest since it is involved in both social behavior and mate choice in different bird species (Bennett et al. 1997; Andersson and Andersson 1998; Siitari et al. 2002; Delhey et al. 2003). The differences between the male and female Monk Parakeet's crown are subtle (see Fig. 2), with values close to the discrimination limit (as in the case of the chest), again making it difficult to gauge the biological significance of the findings. Further experiments under controlled conditions should be carried out in order to evaluate whether the crown is linked to mate choice or other social behavior. Similar results were obtained with the morphometric analysis, where statistically significant differences between the sexes were only observed in terms of bill size and body weight. Again, the differences in bill size and body weight were subtle (just 1 mm in bill size and 5 g between sexes), so it is open to question whether parrots are able to discriminate these differences. Martínez and coworkers (Martinez et al. 2018) found that heavier Monk Parakeet males with larger beaks are paired with heavier females and with larger beaks, but this may be because (similarly to Burrowing Parrot C. patagonus) the Monk Parakeets could form longlasting pair-bonds from an early age and no for assortative mating (Martín and Bucher 1993). The only way to corroborate this idea is by performing experiments under controlled conditions.

    Within the context of the sexual plumage color dimorphism and size dimorphism in birds raised by Owens and Hartley (1998), our results are relevant since they are the first to describe these albeit subtle traits in the Monk Parakeet. In their work, Owens and Hartley studied the relationship between the degree of sexual differences in size, plumage color dimorphism, the kind of mating system, degree of parental care (greater sexual differences in size at higher levels of polygamy and less parental care of the male) and degree of extra-bond paternity displayed by birds (greater sexual differences in plumage coloration at high levels of frequency of extra bond paternity). As already mentioned, Owens and Hartley observed a relation between the sexual plumage color dimorphism for structural coloration (as observed in parrots) and the level of a species' extra-bond paternity. An example of the pattern described by Owens and Hartley is observed in another South American parrot, the Burrowing Parrot (C. patagonus), which is genetically monogamous and practices biparental care (Masello et al. 2002). Only with the use of spectrophotometry could a slight sexual plumage color dimorphism in the green and blue regions (differed in brightness) while the red region differed in spectral shape (reddish ventral patch) and a slight size dimorphism be observed in these birds, males being around 5% larger than females (Masello and Quillfeldt 2003, 2009). This finding corroborates the idea raised by Owens and Hartley (smaller differences in size and more parental care in monogamous species and subtle differences in coloration in species with low frequency of extra-bond paternity). In addition, despite the fact that the fundamental unit of social structure in Monk Parakeet populations is the pair (Hobson et al. 2014, 2015), which would indicate them to be monogamous, the populations studied by Martinez et al. and Bucher et al. show extra-bond paternity (EBP) behaviors, intra-specific parasitism (ISP) and reproductive trios (RT) (Martínez et al. 2013; Bucher et al. 2016). But the fact is that these behaviors in Monk Parakeets occur in a context of high inbreeding (Bucher et al. 2016); that is, the exhibited EBP, ISP and RT behaviors always occur between highly related individuals, resulting in high levels of inbreeding. This inbreeding phenomenon could be partly explained by the low dispersal distance (~ 2 km) of this species in its native South American distribution (Martín and Bucher 1993). The opposite phenomenon is observed when the Monk Parakeet behaves as an invasive exotic species in other regions of the world, where its dispersion distance can reach up to 100 km, favoring a reduction in inbreeding parameters (Da Silva et al. 2010). The high inbreeding values found by Bucher and coworkers could act as an attenuating factor of the sexual dimorphism observed in the Monk Parakeet and could explain the subtle sexual plumage color dimorphism and size dimorphism findings in our work. More detailed studies at the level of coloration and size are required to shed more light on this issue.

    Since the Monk Parakeet appears to exhibit subtle sexual plumage color dimorphism and size dimorphism, the question then arises as to how males and females are able to tell themselves apart in order to form couples. Unlike in the case of the Budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus), where sex differences in cere color and vocalization have been documented (Baltz and Clark 1996; Nespor et al. 1996; Hile et al. 2000), it is not possible to study cere color in the Monk Parakeet because the cere is covered by feathers of the same color as the forehead; and only group vocalizations have been studied within Monk Parakeet colonies during social interactions (threat call, alarm call, flight call, contact call, etc.) (Martella and Bucher 1990). Whether or not there are sex differences in this species' vocalization therefore remains undetermined. Behavioral sex differences have been reported in different species of parrots (repetitive strutting, raised crests, strutting back and forth along a perch, head bowing, flaring the wings, etc.) (del Hoyo et al. 1992), but only repetitive strutting behaviors have been observed in the Monk Parakeet (Eberhard 1998), indicating that it is perhaps a combination of calls and behaviors that make the sexes distinguishable from one another.

    In conclusion, this study reports a subtle sex plumage color dimorphism and size dimorphism in a species of a colonially breeding parrot native to South America, the Monk Parakeet. Comprehensive studies aimed at discriminating sex differences in calls and behaviors should be carried out in order to arrive at a better understanding of sexual recognition in this species.

    We are grateful to the Córdoba Zoo (Argentina) for their generous assistance and for allowing us to conduct part of this research in their facilities (Avian feed, etc.). This work is dedicated to the memory of Mr. Luis Ricardo Valdez for his invaluable assistance with the trap construction. D. J. Valdez and S. M. Benitez-Vieyra are researchers of CONICET. The study met Argentine legal requirements.

    MM and DJG contributed equally to this paper. DJV conceived and designed the experiments. MM, DJG and DJV performed the experiments. MM, DJG, DJV and SMB-V analyzed the data. DJV contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools. DJV and SMB-V wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    The corresponding author (Diego Javier Valdez) had no funding to cover this research; the entire work was made possible by the generous support of Córdoba Zoo, whose staff provided assistance in catching parakeets. PhDs Alicia Sércic and Andrea Cocucci from the Multidisciplinary Institute of Plant Biology (IMBIV-CONICET-UNC) generously lent the spectrophotometer. None of the persons and institutions mentioned above had grants for this research.

    The datasets (data tables and scripts) supporting the conclusions of this article are available upon request.

    This study was carried out in strict accordance with the Guidelines for Ethical Research on Laboratory and Farm Animals and Wildlife Species and with the prior approval of the ethics committee of the Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) (Resolution No. 1047 ANNEXO II, 2005). The required permits were acquired from the Ministerio de Agua, Ambiente y Servicios Públicos of Córdoba, Argentina, through the Secretaría de Ambiente y Cambio Climático to capture specimens of Monk Parakeet for scientific purposes.

    Not applicable.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

  • Altschul SF, Gish W, Miller W, Myers EW, Lipman DJ. Basic local alignment search tool. J Mol Biol. 1990;215:403-10.
    Armstrong MH, Braun EL, Kimball RT. Phylogenetic utility of avian ovomucoid intron G: a comparison of nuclear and mitochondrial phylogenies in the Galliformes. Auk. 2001;118:799-804.
    Berlin S, Ellegren H. Evolutionary genetics: clonal inheritance of avian mitochondrial DNA. Nature. 2001;413:37-8.
    Betancur-R R, Li C, Munroe TA, Ballesteros JA, Ortí G. Addressing gene tree discordance and non-stationarity to resolve a multi-locus phylogeny of the flatfishes (Teleostei: Pleuronectiformes). Syst Biol. 2013;62:763-85.
    Bolívar P, Guéguen L, Duret L, Ellegren H, Mugal CF. GC-biased gene conversion conceals the prediction of the nearly neutral theory in avian genomes. Genome Biol. 2019;20:5.
    Braun EL, Kimball RT. Examining basal avian divergences with mitochondrial sequences: model complexity, taxon sampling and sequence length. Syst Biol. 2002;51:614-25.
    Braun EL, Cracraft J, Houde P. Resolving the avian tree of life from top to bottom: the promise and potential boundaries of the phylogenomic era. In: Kraus RHS, editor. Avian genomics in ecology and evolution—from the lab into the wild. Cham: Springer; 2019. p. 151-210.
    Brown JM, Thomson RC. Bayes factors unmask highly variable information content, bias, and extreme influence in phylogenomic analyses. Syst Biol. 2016;66:517-30.
    Camacho C, Coulouris G, Avagyan V, Ma N, Papadopoulos J, Bealer K, et al. BLAST+: architecture and applications. BMC Bioinform. 2008;10:421.
    Cantarel BL, Korf I, Robb SMC, Parra G, Ross E, Moore B, et al. MAKER: an easy-to-use annotation pipeline designed for emerging model organism genomes. Genome Res. 2008;18:188-96.
    Chen MY, Liang D, Zhang P. Phylogenomic resolution of the phylogeny of laurasiatherian mammals: exploring phylogenetic signals within coding and noncoding sequences. Genome Biol Evol. 2017;9:1998-2012.
    Chojnowski J, Kimball RT, Braun EL. Introns outperform exons in analyses of basal avian phylogeny using clathrin heavy chain genes. Gene. 2008;410:89-96.
    Conant GC, Lewis PO. Effects of nucleotide composition bias on the success of the parsimony criterion in phylogenetic inference. Mol Biol Evol. 2001;18:1024-33.
    Cunningham F, Amode MR, Barrell D, Beal K, Billis K, Brent S, et al. Ensembl 2015. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43:D662-9.
    Doyle JJ. Trees within trees: genes and species, molecules and morphology. Syst Biol. 1997;46:537-53.
    Edgar RC. MUSCLE: multiple sequence alignment with high accuracy and high throughput. Nucleic Acids Res. 2004;32:1792-5.
    Edwards SV, Cloutier A, Baker AJ. Conserved nonexonic elements: a novel class of marker for phylogenetics. Syst Biol. 2017;66:1028-44.
    Eriksson J, Larson G, Gunnarsson U, Bed'hom B, Tixier-Boichard M, Strömstedt L, et al. Identification of the Yellow Skin gene reveals a hybrid origin of the domestic chicken. PLoS Genet. 2008;4:e1000010.
    Faircloth BC, McCormack JE, Crawford NG, Harvey MG, Brumfield RT, Glenn TC. Ultraconserved elements anchor thousands of genetic markers spanning multiple evolutionary timescales. Syst Biol. 2012;61:717-26.
    Felsenstein J. Confidence limits on phylogenies: an approach using the bootstrap. Evolution. 1985;39:783-91.
    Gatesy J, Springer MS. Phylogenetic analysis at deep timescales: unreliable gene trees, bypassed hidden support, and the coalescence/concatalescence conundrum. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2014;80:231-66.
    Gee H. Evolution: ending incongruence. Nature. 2003;425:782.
    Gill F, Donsker D. IOC world bird list (v9.2). 2019. .
    Hill WG, Robertson A. The effect of linkage on limits to artificial selection. Genet Res. 1966;8:269-94.
    Hosner PA, Faircloth BC, Glenn TC, Braun EL, Kimball RT. Avoiding missing data biases in assembling the landfowl tree of life (Aves: Galliformes). Mol Biol Evol. 2016;33:1110-25.
    Hosner PA, Tobias JA, Braun EL, Kimball RT. How do seemingly non-vagile clades accomplish trans-marine dispersal? Trait and dispersal evolution in the landfowl. Proc Roy Soc Lond B. 2017;284:20170210.
    Imsland F, Feng C, Boije H, Bed'Hom B, Fillon V, Dorshorst B, et al. The Rose-comb mutation in chickens constitutes a structural rearrangement causing both altered comb morphology and defective sperm motility. PLoS Genet. 2012;8:e1002775.
    International Chicken Genome Sequencing Consortium (ICGSC). Sequence and comparative analysis of the chicken genome provide unique perspectives on vertebrate evolution. Nature. 2004;432:695-716.
    Jarvis ED, Mirarab S, Aberer AJ, Li B, Houde P, Li C, et al. Whole-genome analyses resolve early branches in the tree of life of modern birds. Science. 2014;346:1320-31.
    Jeffroy O, Brinkmann H, Delsuc F, Philippe H. Phylogenomics: the beginning of incongruence? Trends Genet. 2006;22:225-31.
    Johnsgard PA. The pheasants of the world. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1999. p. 92-9.
    Kan XZ, Li XF, Lei ZP, Chen L, Gao H, Yang ZY, et al. Estimation of divergence times for major lineages of galliform birds: evidence from complete mitochondrial genome sequences. Afric J Biotech. 2010a;9:3073-8.
    Kan XZ, Yang JK, Li XF, Chen L, Lei ZP, Wang M, et al. Phylogeny of major lineages of Galliform birds (Aves: Galliformes) based on complete mitochondrial genomes. Genet Mol Res. 2010b;9:1625-33.
    Katoh K, Standley DM. MAFFT multiple sequence alignment software version 7: improvements in performance and usability. Mol Biol Evol. 2013;30:772-80.
    Katzman S, Kern AD, Bejerano G, Fewell G, Fulton L, Wilson RK, et al. Human genome ultraconserved elements are ultraselected. Science. 2007;317:915.
    Kimball RT, Braun EL. A multigene phylogeny of Galliformes supports a single origin of erectile ability in non-feathered facial traits. J Avian Biol. 2008;39:438-45.
    Kimball RT, Braun EL. Does more sequence data improve estimates of galliform phylogeny? Analyses of a rapid radiation using a complete data matrix. PeerJ. 2014;2:e361.
    Kimball RT, Braun EL, Zwartjes P, Crowe TM, Ligon JD. A molecular phylogeny of the pheasants and partridges suggests that these lineages are not monophyletic. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 1999;11:38-54.
    Kimball RT, Mary CM, Braun EL. A macroevolutionary perspective on multiple sexual traits in the Phasianidae (Galliformes). Int J Evol Biol. 2011;2011:423938.
    Kimball RT, Wang N, Heimer-McGinn V, Ferguson C, Braun EL. Identifying localized biases in large datasets: a case study using the avian tree of life. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2013;69:1021-32.
    Lawal RA, Al-Atiyat RM, Aljumaah RS, Silva P, Mwacharo JM, Hanotte O. Whole-genome resequencing of red junglefowl and indigenous village chicken reveal new insights on the genome dynamics of the species. Front Genet. 2018;9:264.
    Li D, Li Y, Li M, Che T, Tian S, Chen B, et al. Population genomics identifies patterns of genetic diversity and selection in chicken. BMC Genomics. 2019;20:263.
    Li L, Stoeckert J Jr, Roos DS. OrthoMCL: identification of ortholog groups for eukaryotic genomes. Genome Res. 2003;13:2178-89.
    Ligon JD, Kimball RT, Merola-Zwartjes M. Mate choice in red junglefowl: the issues of multiple ornaments and fluctuating asymmetry. Anim Behav. 1998;55:41-50.
    Maddison WP. Gene trees in species trees. Syst Biol. 1997;46:523-36.
    Marcais G, Kingsford C. A fast, lock-free approach for efficient parallel counting of occurrences of k-mers. Bioninformatics. 2011;27:764-70.
    Marcais G, Yorke JA, Zimin A. QuorUM: an error corrector for Illumina reads. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0130821.
    McCarthy EM. Handbook of avian hybrids of the world. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006.
    Meiklejohn KA, Danielson MJ, Braun EL, Faircloth BC, Glenn TC, Kimball RT. Incongruence among different mitochondrial regions: a case study using complete mitogenomes. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2014;78:314-23.
    Meiklejohn KA, Faircloth BC, Glenn TC, Kimball RT, Braun EL. Analysis of a rapid evolutionary radiation using ultraconserved elements: evidence for a bias in some multispecies coalescent methods. Syst Biol. 2016;65:612-27.
    Mendes FK, Hahn MW. Gene tree discordance causes apparent substitution rate variation. Syst Biol. 2017;65:711-21.
    Mirarab S, Bayzid MS, Boussau B, Warnow T. Statistical binning improves species tree estimation in the presence of gene tree incongruence. Science. 2014;346:1250463.
    Moore WS. Inferring phylogenies from mtDNA variation: mitochondrial-gene trees versus nuclear-gene trees. Evolution. 1995;49:718-26.
    Mugal CF, Weber CC, Ellegren H. GC-biased gene conversion links the recombination landscape and demography to genomic base composition: gC-biased gene conversion drives genomic base composition across a wide range of species. BioEssays. 2015;37:1317-26.
    Nishibori M, Shimogiri T, Hayashi T, Yasue H. Molecular evidence for hybridization of species in the genus Gallus except for Gallus varius. Anim Genet. 2005;36:367-75.
    Pamilo P, Nei M. Relationships between gene trees and species trees. Mol Biol Evol. 1988;5:568-83.
    Patel S, Kimball RT, Braun EL. Error in phylogenetic estimation for bushes in the tree of life. J Phylogenet Evol Biol. 2013;1:110.
    Peterson AT, Brisbin IL. Genetic endangerment of wild Red Junglefowl Gallus gallus? Bird Conserv Int. 1998;8:387-94.
    Reddy S, Kimball RT, Pandey A, Hosner PA, Braun MJ, Hackett SJ, et al. Why do phylogenomic data sets yield conflicting trees? data type influences the avian tree of life more than taxon sampling. Syst Biol. 2017;66:857-79.
    Rosenberg NA. The probability of topological concordance of gene trees and species trees. Theor Pop Biol. 2002;61:225-47.
    Saitou N, Nei M. The number of nucleotides required to determine the branching order of three species, with special reference to the human-chimpanzee-gorilla divergence. J Mol Evol. 1986;24:189-204.
    Sayyari E, Mirarab S. Fast coalescent-based computation of local branch support from quartet frequencies. Mol Biol Evol. 2016;33:1654-68.
    Shen XX, Hittinger CT, Rokas A. Contentious relationships in phylogenomic studies can be driven by a handful of genes. Nat Ecol Evol. 2017;1:0126.
    Shen YY, Dai K, Cao X, Murphy RW, Shen XJ, Zhang YP. The updated phylogenies of the Phasianidae based on combined data of nuclear and mitochondrial DNA. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e95786.
    Shen YY, Liang L, Sun YB, Yue BS, Yang XJ, Murphy RW, et al. A mitogenomic perspective on the ancient, rapid radiation in the Galliformes with an emphasis on the Phasianidae. BMC Evol Biol. 2010;10:132.
    Smit AFA, Hubley R, Green P. RepeatMasker Open-4.0. . Accessed 8 Dec 2015.
    Solís-Lemus C, Ané C. Inferring phylogenetic networks with maximum pseudolikelihood under incomplete lineage sorting. PLoS Genet. 2016;12:e1005896.
    Solís-Lemus C, Bastide P, Ané C. PhyloNetworks: a package for phylogenetic networks. Mol Biol Evol. 2017;34:3292-8.
    Song S, Liu L, Edwards SV, Wu S. Resolving conflict in eutherian mammal phylogeny using phylogenomics and the multispecies coalescent model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:14942-7.
    Springer MS, Gatesy J. The gene tree delusion. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2016;94:1-33.
    Springer MS, Gatesy J. Retroposon insertions within a multispecies coalescent framework suggest that ratite phylogeny is not in the 'Anomaly Zone'. bioRxiv. 2019. .
    Stamatakis A. RAxML version 8: a tool for phylogenetic analysis and post-analysis of large phylogenies. Bioinformatics. 2014;30:1312-3.
    Stanke M, Schöffmann O, Morgenstern B, Waack S. Gene prediction in eukaryotes with a generalized hidden Markov model that uses hints from external sources. BMC Bioinform. 2006;7:62.
    Stein RW, Brown JW, Mooers AØ. A molecular genetic time scale demonstrates Cretaceous origins and multiple diversification rate shifts within the order Galliformes (Aves). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2015;92:155-64.
    Swofford DL. PAUP*: phylogenetic analysis using parsimony (*and other methods). Version 4.0a166. 2019.
    Tamashiro RA, White ND, Braun MJ, Faircloth BC, Braun EL, Kimball RT. What are the roles of taxon sampling and model fit in tests of cyto-nuclear discordance using avian mitogenomic data? Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2019;130:132-42.
    Tiley GP, Kimball RT, Braun EL, Burleigh JG. Comparison of the Chinese Bamboo Partridge and Red Junglefowl genome sequences highlights the importance of demography in genome evolution. BMC Genomics. 2018;19:336.
    Wang N, Kimball RT, Braun EL, Liang B, Zhang Z. Assessing phylogenetic relationships among Galliformes: a multigene phylogeny with expanded taxon sampling in Phasianidae. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e64312.
    Wang N, Kimball RT, Braun EL, Liang B, Zhang Z. Ancestral range reconstruction of Galliformes: the effects of topology and taxon sampling. J Biogeogr. 2017;44:122-35.
    Wang MS, Li Y, Peng MS, Zhong L, Wang ZJ, Li QY, et al. Genomic analyses reveal potential independent adaptation to high altitude in Tibetan chickens. Mol Biol Evol. 2015;32:1880-9.
    Webster MT, Axelsson E, Ellegren H. Strong regional biases in nucleotide substitution in the chicken genome. Mol Biol Evol. 2006;23:1203-16.
    Wheeler TJ, Eddy SR. nhmmer: DNA homology search with profile HMMs. Bioinformatics. 2013;29:2487-9.
    Xi Z, Liu L, Davis CC. Genes with minimal phylogenetic information are problematic for coalescent analyses when gene tree estimation is biased. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2015;92:63-71.
    Xu B, Yang Z. Challenges in species tree estimation under the multispecies coalescent model. Genetics. 2016;204:1353-68.
    Yang S, Shi Z, Ou X, Liu G. Whole-genome resequencing reveals genetic indels of feathered-leg traits in domestic chickens. J Genet. 2019;98:47.
    Yi G, Qu L, Liu J, Yan Y, Xu G, Yang N. Genome-wide patterns of copy number variation in the diversified chicken genomes using next-generation sequencing. BMC Genom. 2014;15:962.
    Zhang C, Rabiee M, Sayyari E, Mirarab S. ASTRAL-Ⅲ: polynomial time species tree reconstruction from partially resolved gene trees. BMC Bioinform. 2018;19:153.
    Zimin AV, Marcais G, Puiu D, Roberts M, Salzberg SL, Yorke JA. The MaSuRCA genome assembler. Bioinformatics. 2013;29:2669-77.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(14)

    1. Eun Sub Kim, Dong Kun Lee, Jiyoung Choi. Evaluating the effectiveness of mitigation measures in environmental impact assessments: A comprehensive review of development projects in Korea. Heliyon, 2024, 10(11): e31647. DOI:10.1016/j.heliyon.2024.e31647
    2. Binod Bhattarai, Dipendra Adhikari, Jagan Nath Adhikari, et al. Effects of invasive alien plants and habitat structure on bird assemblages in a unique lowland forest in eastern Nepal. Ornis Hungarica, 2024, 32(2): 1. DOI:10.2478/orhu-2024-0019
    3. Daichi Iijima, Takeshi Osawa. Seasonal changes in altitudinal patterns of bird species richness in a temperate low-elevation mountain. Acta Oecologica, 2024, 125: 104033. DOI:10.1016/j.actao.2024.104033
    4. Yihenew Aynalem, Afework Bekele, Bezawork Afework Bogale, et al. Population Status and Threats to Galliform Bird in the Arjo-Didessa River Valley, Southwestern Ethiopia. International Journal of Zoology, 2023, 2023: 1. DOI:10.1155/2023/5730010
    5. Nasiphi Bitani, Craig P. Cordier, David A. Ehlers Smith, et al. Microhabitat requirements and occupancy of understorey bird forest specialists in Southern Mistbelt Forests of KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Forest Ecology and Management, 2023, 549: 121484. DOI:10.1016/j.foreco.2023.121484
    6. Janaki Paudel, Laxman Khanal, Naresh Pandey, et al. Determinants of Herpetofaunal Diversity in a Threatened Wetland Ecosystem: A Case Study of the Ramaroshan Wetland Complex, Western Nepal. Animals, 2022, 13(1): 135. DOI:10.3390/ani13010135
    7. Laxmi Prasad Upadhyaya, Naresh Pandey, Laxman Khanal. Tribhuvan University area serves as a greenspace for birds in the Kathmandu Valley, Central Nepal. Journal of Animal Diversity, 2022, 4(1): 27. DOI:10.52547/JAD.2022.4.1.4
    8. Bijaya Neupane, Bijaya Dhami, Shristee Panthee, et al. Forest Management Practice Influences Bird Diversity in the Mid-Hills of Nepal. Animals, 2022, 12(19): 2681. DOI:10.3390/ani12192681
    9. Dilshod Akhrorov, Tianlong Cai, Gang Song, et al. Ecological constraints on elevational gradients of bird species richness in Tajikistan. Avian Research, 2022, 13: 100026. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100026
    10. Sudesh Kumar, Asha Sohil, Muzaffar A. Kichloo, et al. Landscape heterogeneity affects diurnal raptor communities in a sub-tropical region of northwestern Himalayas, India. PLOS ONE, 2022, 17(4): e0246555. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0246555
    11. Aadil Hussain Bhat, Altaf Hussain Mir, Samina Amin Charoo. Influence of habitat heterogeneity on the elevational distribution and guild structure of birds in the Rajparian Wildlife Sanctuary, Kashmir Himalaya. Ornithology Research, 2022, 31(1): 51. DOI:10.1007/s43388-022-00111-3
    12. Bishow Poudel, Bijaya Neupane, Rajeev Joshi, et al. Factors affecting the species richness and composition of bird species in a community managed forest of Nepal. Journal of Threatened Taxa, 2021, 13(9): 19212. DOI:10.11609/jott.6945.13.9.19212-19222
    13. Priyanka Chandna. DIVERSITY AND RESIDENTIAL STATUS OF WADER BIRDS AT HODAL IN PALWAL DISTRICT, IN HARYANA, INDIA.. INDIAN JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH, 2021. DOI:10.36106/ijar/2315406
    14. Juna Neupane, Laxman Khanal, Basant Gyawali, et al. Elevational pattern and seasonality of avian diversity in Kaligandaki River Basin, central Himalaya. Journal of Threatened Taxa, 2020, 12(14): 16927. DOI:10.11609/jott.5815.12.14.16927-16943

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(5)  /  Tables(5)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (1534) PDF downloads (20) Cited by(14)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return