Yang Li, Canwei Xia, Huw Lloyd, Donglai Li, Yanyun Zhang. 2017: Identification of vocal individuality in male cuckoos using different analytical techniques. Avian Research, 8(1): 21. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-017-0079-0
Citation: Yang Li, Canwei Xia, Huw Lloyd, Donglai Li, Yanyun Zhang. 2017: Identification of vocal individuality in male cuckoos using different analytical techniques. Avian Research, 8(1): 21. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-017-0079-0

Identification of vocal individuality in male cuckoos using different analytical techniques

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Canwei Xia, xiacanwei@bnu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 18 Jan 2017
  • Accepted Date: 31 Jul 2017
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 20 Aug 2017
  • Background 

    Individuality in vocalizations may provide an effective tool for surveying populations of the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) but there remains few data on which technique to use to identify individuality. In this research, we compared the within- and between-individual variation in cuckoo calls using two different analytical methods, and discuss the feasibility of using call individuality to count male cuckoos within a population.

    Methods 

    We recorded vocalization from 13 males, and measured 15 spectro-temporal variables for each call. The majority of these call variables (n = 12) have greater variation between individuals than within individual. We first calculated the similarity (Pearson's R) for each paired calls in order to find a threshold that could distinguish calls emitted from the same or different males, and then counted the number of males based on this distinction. Second, we used the more widely accepted technique of discriminant function analysis (DFA) to identify individual male cuckoos, and compared the correct rate of classifying individuals between the two analytical methods.

    Results 

    Similarity of paired calls from the same male was significantly higher than from different males. Under a relatively broad threshold interval, we achieved a high (> 90%) correct rate to distinguish calls and an accurate estimate of male numbers. Based on banded males (n = 3), we found the similarity of paired calls from different days was lower when compared with paired calls from the same day, but this change did not obscure individual identification, as similarity values of paired calls from different days were still larger than the threshold used to distinguish calls from the same or different males. DFA also yielded a high rate (91.9%) of correct classification of individuals.

    Conclusions 

    Our study suggests that identifying individual vocalizations can form the basis of an appropriate survey method for counting male cuckoos within a population, provided the performance of different analytical techniques are compared.

  • Mothers have a variety of ways to manipulate the composition and/or environment of their eggs, including, but not limited to: hatching asynchrony (Clark and Wilson 1981; Magrath 1990), yolk hormones (Groothuis et al. 2005), sex allocation (Pike and Petrie 2003), egg size (Krist 2011), incubation temperature (DuRant et al. 2009, 2011), and carotenoids (Royle et al. 2001; Blount et al. 2002). Assessing how these factors influence the growth and survival of their offspring, however, first requires being able to accurately match hatchlings to their respective eggs. This can be a challenging endeavor for many bird species.

    Frequent nest checks can be employed to directly observe the individual hatching of eggs, although increased disturbance at the nest may lead to higher rates of abandonment or predation. Even the most frequent nest checks, however, can result in finding two hatchlings within the same nest. Because egg and hatchling mass are significantly correlated in most bird species (Williams 1994; Deeming and Birchard 2007), simply assigning the heaviest hatchling to the heaviest egg (i.e., relative mass assignment; RMA) in these situations could be tempting, as RMA would increase sample sizes and reduce the number of nest checks required.

    Here I present RMA as a new technique, at least to my knowledge, that may be useful in studies of maternal effects, and determine the accuracy of RMA by applying the method retrospectively to a dataset where Common Grackle (Quiscalus quiscula) hatchlings were assigned to eggs unambiguously. Common Grackles are an appropriate species in which to test the utility of RMA because hatchling mass is correlated with egg mass (Howe 1976), egg size is variable both within and among clutches (Howe 1976; Maddox and Weatherhead 2008), and 2‒4 eggs within a given clutch can hatch at approximately the same time (Howe 1976; Maddox and Weatherhead 2008).

    I used an incubator with individual compartments to unambiguously assign hatchlings to their respective eggs as part of a larger study investigating maternal effects in Common Grackles. Detailed descriptions of the study site and field methods can be found in Maddox and Weatherhead(2008, 2009, 2012). Briefly, I found nests under construction and monitored them daily until fledging. On the day each egg was laid, I measured its mass (±0.1 g) and numbered it sequentially with a nontoxic felt-tip marker. When I observed at least one egg pipping from an individual nest during my daily nest checks, I collected all the eggs from the clutch and placed each egg in an individual compartment within the incubator. In total, eggs from 133 nests were treated as such. Artificial eggs of similar size and appearance were placed in nests to limit female abandonment. Upon hatching I measured the mass (±0.1 g), wing chord (±0.1 mm) and tarsus (±0.1 mm) of each hatchling before returning it to its respective nest.

    When more than one egg hatched between checks of the incubator or approximately every 1‒2 h, I used RMA to assign hatchlings to their respective eggs by rank-ordering the mass of hatchlings and their candidate eggs separately and assigning hatchlings to eggs of the same rank (i.e., 1-1, 2-2, etc.). Because I knew the source egg in each case, I could evaluate the error rate of RMA. Typically two eggs from a given clutch hatched between checks but finding 3‒4 hatchlings was not uncommon. This is essentially identical to what occurs in the field with frequent nest checks.

    In 78 of the 133 clutches examined, two eggs hatched between consecutive checks of the incubator. RMA correctly matched 122 (78.2%) hatchlings to their respective eggs, whereas 14 (9.0%) hatchlings were incorrectly assigned. RMA could not be applied to 20 (12.8%) hatchlings because the mass of the two hatchlings or the two candidate eggs was identical. For those egg-hatchling dyads that were assigned correctly, paired differences of egg and hatchling masses averaged 0.40 g ± 0.03 SE (range 0.1-0.9 g, n = 61) and 0.38 g ± 0.04 SE (range 0.1-1.4 g, n = 61), respectively. For the 14 egg-hatchling dyads that were assigned incorrectly, paired average differences of egg (0.23 g ± 0.07 SE; range 0.1-0.6 g) and hatchling (0.21 g ± 0.04 SE; range 0.1-0.4 g) masses were almost half that of correctly assigned dyads. Excluding those hatchlings that could not be assigned, the error rate of RMA when applied to two hatchlings was 10.3% (14/136).

    In 50 of the 133 clutches, three eggs hatched between consecutive incubator checks. RMA correctly matched 66 (44%) hatchlings to their respective eggs, whereas 5 clutches (10%) had at least one incorrectly assigned hatchling. Twenty-three (46%) nests contained at least one hatchling that could not be assigned to an egg for the reasons indicated above. Differences in egg mass of correct assignments averaged 0.25 ± 0.03 SE, whereas wrong assignments averaged 0.26 ± 0.02 SE. Excluding unassigned hatchlings, the error rate of RMA was 7.0% (5/71).

    In the remaining nine clutches, 4 eggs within the same nest hatched between incubator checks. In one (14%) clutch, all 4 hatchlings were assigned correctly to their eggs. In another (14%) clutch, at least one hatchling was incorrectly assigned. In 7 (78%) clutches, at least one hatchling could not be assigned to an egg for the reasons indicated above. Excluding unassigned hatchlings, the error rate of RMA was 50% (1/2).

    RMA was largely successful at correctly assigning hatchlings to their respective eggs. The ability of RMA to assign eggs, however, was highly dependent on the number of hatchlings present between checks. When applied to ≥3 unknown egg-hatchling dyads the percentage of hatchlings that RMA was unable to assign was substantial, essentially eliminating its use in those situations. Consequently, I focus the remaining discussion to those cases where only two hatchlings were found between checks. With only two hatchlings present, RMA may prove to be a useful technique in some species, but its widespread adoption will likely be restricted by several shortcomings.

    The accuracy of RMA is likely to vary directly with the extent that egg and hatchling mass are correlated. RMA will almost certainly perform below acceptable levels when egg mass explains little of the variation in hatchling mass. Indeed, those dyads that were incorrectly assigned generally had greater residuals than correctly assigned dyads (Fig. 1). Given that egg mass explained 82% of the variation in hatchling mass in Common Grackles—a high value for most bird species (Williams 1994)—and yet still incurred a 10% error rate, RMA should be limited to species in which egg and hatchling mass are highly correlated.

    Figure  1.  Relative mass assignment performed better when the difference in egg mass was greater with incorrectly assigned eggs generally having greater residuals than correctly assigned dyads. Colors indicate correct (green), incorrect (red), or unknown (blue) assignments of egg-hatchling dyads

    Restricting the application of RMA by a pre-established minimum difference in egg or hatchling mass may improve the error rate, because the pair-wise difference in mass was smaller for incorrectly assigned egg-hatchling dyads than those that were correctly assigned (Fig. 1). Limiting RMA in the current dataset to only eggs that differed in mass by > 0.1 g, reduced the error rate in half to 5%, but doubled the percentage of hatchlings that were unable to be assigned from 13 to 27%. Limiting the assignment of hatchlings had no discernible effect.

    A potential pitfall of RMA not accounted for in the present study is the ability of parents to feed hatchlings between nest checks. Given the small difference in mass between nest mates, any amount of food given differentially to hatchlings could alter the mass difference relative to their nest mate and thus potentially result in a wrong assignment. This may prove negligible in many species, especially in species where males provide little parental care, as it is likely that females would still be brooding hatchlings and/or incubating eggs. Common Grackle hatchlings, however, started begging for food immediately after hatching, and I did occasionally observe males feeding hatchlings. Consequently, the error rate I report here is likely a best-case scenario.

    One modification to RMA that may improve its accuracy that the current study was unable to assess is to measure egg mass directly before hatching. Given that egg mass decreases during incubation due to water loss as a result of embryonic development (Ar and Rahn 1980), egg mass obtained after this mass loss in addition to or instead of fresh egg mass may decrease the number of incorrect assignments. Future studies will need to verify if this modification would markedly increase the accuracy of RMA.

    RMA may prove to be a valuable tool for studies that require known egg-hatchling dyads, although its accuracy should first be tested on the species in which it will ultimately be applied. Its successful application will likely be limited to those species where egg and hatchling mass are highly correlated or within-clutch egg-size variation is large. Researchers should be cognizant of the fact that RMA is a one-tailed error—egg-size effects will always be inflated—and thus could potentially result in erroneously reporting significant effects when in fact they do not exist.

    I am indebted to the Shoemaker family for allowing me to work on their land and to the many people who contributed to this project over the years, especially K. Kopatz, C. Johnson, D. Kikuchi, and P. Wesley. Funding was provided by the University of Illinois, Program in Ecology, Evolution and Conservation, and the American Ornithologists' Union Research Award. L.S. Johnson provided useful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. I was supported by an NSF International Fellowship OISE-1159178 while preparing an earlier version of this manuscript.

    The author declares that he has no competing interests.

    All work described here was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the University of Illinois (protocol #05006).

  • Budka M, Wojas L, Osiejuk TS. Is it possible to acoustically identify individuals within a population? J Ornithol. 2015;156:1-8.
    Charrier I, Mathevon N, Jouventin P, Aubin T. Acoustic communication in a black headed gull colony: how do chicks identify their parents? Ethology. 2001;107:961-74.
    Charrier I, Mathevon N, Jouventin P. Individuality in the voice of fur seal females: an analysis study of the pup attraction call in Arctocephalus tropicalis. Mar Mamm Sci. 2003;19:161-72.
    Dawson DK, Efford MG. Bird population density estimated from acoustic signals. J Appl Ecol. 2009;46:1201-9.
    Erritzøe J, Mann CF, Brammer FP, Fuller RA. Cuckoos of the world. Bloomsbury: Christopher Helm; 2012.
    Fox EJS. A new perspective on acoustic individual recognition in animals with limited call sharing or changing repertoires. Anim Behav. 2008;75:1187-94.
    Frommolt KH, Tauchert KH. Applying bioacoustic methods for long-term monitoring of a nocturnal wetland bird. Ecol Inf. 2014;21:4-12.
    Fuisz TI, de Kort SR. Habitat-dependent call divergence in the common cuckoo: is it a potential signal for assortative mating? Proc R Soc Lond B Biol. 2007;274:2093-7.
    Gilbert G, McGregor P, Tyler G. Vocal individuality as a census tool: Practical considerations illustrated by a study of two rare species. J Field Ornithol. 1994;65:335-48.
    Jung WJ, Lee JW, Yoo JC. "cu-coo": can you recognize my stepparents? A study of host-specific male call divergence in the common cuckoo. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e90468.
    Li DL, Ruan YN, Wang Y, Chang AK, Wan DM, Zhang ZW. Egg-spot matching in common cuckoo parasitism of the oriental reed warbler: effects of host nest availability and egg rejection. Avian Res. 2016;7:21.
    Møller AP, Morelli F, Mousseau TA, Tryjanowski P. The number of syllables in chernobyl cuckoo calls reliably indicate habitat, soil and radiation levels. Ecol Ind. 2016a;66:592-7.
    Møller AP, Morelli F, Tryjanowski P. Cuckoo folklore and human well-being: cuckoo calls predict how long farmers live. Ecol Ind. 2016b;72:766-8.
    Morelli F, Jiguet F, Reif J, Plexida S, Valli AS, Indykiewicz P, Simova P, Tichit M, Moretti M, Tryjanowski P. Cuckoo and biodiversity: testing the correlation between species occurrence and bird species richness in Europe. Biol Conserv. 2015;190:123-32.
    R Core Development Team. R: a Language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. .
    Robisson P, Aubin T, Bremond JC. Individuality in the voice of the emperor penguin Aptenodytes forsteri: adaptation to a noisy environment. Ethology. 1993;94:279-90.
    Tryjanowski P, Morelli F. Presence of cuckoo reliably indicates high bird diversity: a case study in a farmland area. Ecol Ind. 2015;55:52-8.
    Vogl W, Taborsky B, Taborsky M, Teuschl Y, Honza M. Habitat and space use of European cuckoo females during the egg laying period. Behaviour. 2004;141:881-98.
    Wei CT, Jia CX, Dong L, Wang DP, Xia CW, Zhang YY, Liang W. Geographic variation in the calls of the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus): isolation by distance and divergence among subspecies. J Ornithol. 2015;156:533-42.
    Williams BK, Titus K. Assessment of sampling stability in ecological applications of discriminant analysis. Ecology. 1988;69:1275-85.
    Williams HM, Willemoes M, Klaassen RH, Strandberg R, Thorup K. Common cuckoo home ranges are larger in the breeding season than in the non-breeding season and in regions of sparse forest cover. J Ornithol. 2015;157:461-9.
    Xia CW, Xiao H, Zhang YY. Individual variation in brownish-flanked bush warbler songs. The Condor. 2010;112:591-5.
    Xia CW, Huang R, Wei CC, Nie PW, Zhang YY. Individual identification on the basis of the songs of the asian stubtail (urosphena squameiceps). Chin Birds. 2011;2:132-9.
    Zsebők S, Moskát C, Bán M. Individually distinctive vocalization in common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus). J Ornithol. 2017;158:213-22.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(1)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (255) PDF downloads (20) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return