Melissah Rowe, Simon C. Griffith, Antje Hofgaard, Jan T. Lifjeld. 2015: Subspecific variation in sperm morphology and performance in the Long-tailed Finch (Poephila acuticauda). Avian Research, 6(1): 23. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0032-z
Citation: Melissah Rowe, Simon C. Griffith, Antje Hofgaard, Jan T. Lifjeld. 2015: Subspecific variation in sperm morphology and performance in the Long-tailed Finch (Poephila acuticauda). Avian Research, 6(1): 23. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0032-z

Subspecific variation in sperm morphology and performance in the Long-tailed Finch (Poephila acuticauda)

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Melissah Rowe, melissah.rowe@nhm.uio.no

  • Received Date: 07 Jul 2015
  • Accepted Date: 14 Oct 2015
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 30 Nov 2015
  • Background 

    Evolutionary biology endeavours to explain biological diversity,and as such it is critical to develop an understanding of the adaptive and functional significance of trait variation. Spermatozoa exhibit remarkable levels of morphological diversification. However,our understanding of the evolutionary causes and functional significance of this variation is limited,especially at the intraspecific level.

    Methods 

    We quantified variation in sperm morphology and performance between two subspecies of Long-tailed Finch (Poephila acuticauda acuticauda and P. a. hecki),a small grassfinch found in tropical northern Australia. Despite a zone of secondary contact,these subspecies are maintained as two distinct forms: P. a. acuticauda occurs in the western part of the species' range and has a yellow bill,while P. a. hecki exhibits a red bill and is found in the eastern part of the range.

    Results 

    We found small,but significant differences in sperm size between these subspecies (P. a. acuticauda had longer and narrower sperm than P. a. hecki),which was surprising given the recent evolutionary origins of these two taxa (i.e. 0.3 million years ago). Additionally,both subspecies exhibited high values of between- and within-male variation in sperm morphology,though in the case of sperm midpiece length this variation was significantly lower in P. a. acuticauda relative to P. a. hecki.

    Conclusions 

    We suggest these observed differences in sperm morphology are the result of genetic drift and reflect historical processes associated with divergence between the eastern and western populations of these two subspecies. Finally,we discuss the potential implications of our findings for the process of population divergence and reproductive isolation.

  • Increasing urbanization worldwide has generated both greater awareness of the need to conserve urban biodiversity and an increase in the research required to inform that conservation process (Dunn et al. 2006; DeStefano and Degraaf 2008). Urbanization dramatically changes the natural environment; some species thrive under the altered circumstances ('urban exploiters' and 'adapters'), but others ('urban avoiders') are unable to adjust to the altered environment (Blair 2001). We need to understand what determines this disparity in order to successfully conserve urban biodiversity and ensure the survival of species sensitive to urbanization. One key to successful urban living by birds is an ability to tolerate high levels of disturbance by mostly harmless people (Price 2008). Urban living would not be viable, because critical activities would be curtailed, if birds: (a) spent a large proportion of the day monitoring their environment for stimuli posing little real threat to their survival, or (b) fled long distances rapidly in an energetically-expensive manner when approaching humans were still a long distance away (Frid and Dill 2002).

    Theoretically, the tolerance of human proximity necessary to facilitate urban living could eventuate in one of several ways: (a) cities might only be colonized by bird species whose members are all relatively bold as a result of natural selection for boldness in the exurban environment, (b) urban members of a colonizing species might rapidly become bolder than ex-urban con-specifics through genetic selection (Møller 2008), (c) cities might only be settled by bolder subsets of a species' members through a sorting process (Atwell et al. 2012), or (d) birds that colonize cities might adjust to high human population densities through behavioural plasticity that allows them to rapidly learn that urban humans mostly pose little threat and do not warrant extensive surveillance or early, rapid fleeing responses (Lima and Bednekoff 1999; Weston et al. 2012).

    Species that have genetically adapted to cities for a large number of generations should have shorter Flight Initiation Distances (FID) and smaller relative time allocations to anti-predator vigilance than recent urban colonists. By genetic adaptation we mean adjustment to inhabiting the urban environment that comes about through natural selection (i.e. beneficial genetic change). Møller (2008) demonstrated such a trend for FID in some Western European bird species, but few other studies have addressed this issue, particularly in Australia where cities tend to have a shorter history than many European conurbations. Even where tolerance of disturbing stimulation by humans in cities is primarily achieved through learning, if intra-specific observational, rather than experiental, learning is involved, we might still expect that a very recent urban colonist species would be less tolerant of human proximity than more established species. This would be so because the abundance of models from which to learn tolerance would presumably be relatively low during the colonization process. In birds, FID is a useful, easily-obtained metric of boldness and tolerance of disturbance by humans, with both theoretical and management significance (Weston et al. 2012; Møller et al. 2014; Vines and Lill 2015). Vigilance effort has been used less often in this context, but has potential as a tolerance metric (Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder 2003).

    The Rock Dove (Columba livia) is believed to have been introduced to Australia around the time of British settlement in 1788, but the first recorded releases in the state of Victoria were in the 1870s (Long 1981). It is an 'urban exploiter' that lives commensally with humans in cities in Australia and worldwide (Robbins 1995; Higgins and Davies 1996). Human food waste spillage and food deliberately provided by humans ('volunteer' food) comprise a significant proportion of its urban diet (Mulhall and Lill 2011). The Spotted Dove (Streptopelia chinensis), another urban exploiter, was possibly introduced to Melbourne in the 1860s, but definitely released in the city from 1870-1874 (Long 1981). It depends less on human food waste than the Rock Dove, but also consumes livestock food (Frith et al. 1976; Mulhall and Lill 2011). In contrast, the endemic Crested Pigeon (Ochyphaps lophotes) is an 'urban adapter' that is less commensal than the other two dove species and consumes only a small volume of anthropogenic food (Frith et al. 1974). It has settled in most major east coast temperate zone cities, but only colonized Melbourne's eastern suburbs in the last 15-20 years (A. Lill, personal observation).

    We predicted that in Melbourne the Crested Pigeon would be less adjusted to human proximity than the Rock Dove and Spotted Dove. Not only have the latter two species been established in urban Melbourne far longer than the Crested Pigeon, but the original founder individuals of at least the Rock Dove came from overseas populations that already had a substantial history of close contact with humans. We tested this prediction by contemporaneously: (a) comparing the FID and retreat behaviour of members of the three species to an approaching observer, and (b) measuring the mean proportion of daylight hours spent by the three species in vigilance (i.e. surveillance for threatening stimuli). We reasoned that the predicted lesser tolerance of disturbance by humans of Crested Pigeons would be manifested in fleeing from approaching people earlier than the other species and in spending a greater proportion of terrestrial foraging time in surveillance for approaching humans.

    The study was conducted in the austral autumn and winter (April-August, 2011) at 150 sites spread widely throughout the eastern suburbs of Melbourne, Australia (37.840S, 144.980E). The city has a human population of over 4 million and extends over 9, 900 km2. The study sites, mainly in streets, public parks, recreation reserves and on sports fields, were at least 1 km apart to preclude repeated sampling of unmarked individuals.

    Observations were made on all days of the week between 07:00-11:00 and 13:00-17:00 AEST. At each haphazardly chosen site, two observations per species on two different focal individuals were made for each behavioural variable, whenever possible. Only birds that were on the ground and active were recorded. In conjunction with the behavioural records, we also recorded categorically (0, 1-5 or > 5) several co-variates within 10 m of the focal bird that could potentially influence its behaviour: (1) refuges from predators (e.g. buildings or vegetation cover), (2) potentially threatening stimuli (e.g. predators or moving vehicles), (3) humans and (4) con-specifics and hetero-specific pigeons. Size of the flock of which the focal bird was a member was also noted. All these co-variates have been demonstrated to influence FID in various bird species (Blumstein 2006; Weston et al. 2012). Based on Australian Bureau of Statistics (2011) data, sites were roughly categorized as having an estimated low (≤500 people/km2) or high (> 2, 500 people/km2) human population density.

    The same observer conducted all FID measurements. She always approached the focal bird in a straight line at a steady pace (0.5-1 m per sec) and measured the distance (±1 m) between herself and the bird at which her approach stimulated it to retreat. The Starting Distance (distance from observer's starting point to focal bird's initial location) was also recorded. These distances were measured with a Bushnell Yardage Pro Sport 450™ laser rangefinder. The focal birds' mode of retreat (walking, running or flying) and the (unequal) categorical distances that they retreated (≤5, 5.1-10 and > 10 m) were also recorded. The second recording from a different flock-member was made at least 1 min after the first one to avoid 'carry-over' effects.

    Time investment by foraging doves in vigilance (head-up, visual scanning) was measured for focal individuals over 1-5 min periods using instantaneous sampling (Martin and Bateson 2007) with a 5-sec fixed interval. Co-variates were recorded as outlined above. A second recording from a different member of the same flock was made at least 30-sec after the first one. Although this could potentially reduce independence of the data, there are precedents for using this procedure for constructing time-activity budgets in flocking species (e.g. Magrath and Lill 1983). Vigilance records were obtained from a distance and at a time when the observer's presence or activity were unlikely to be influencing the focal bird's behaviour (McGiffin et al. 2013).

    Statistical analysis was conducted with R version 2.10.1. Data were inspected for conformity with the assumptions of the various significance tests used. To increase statistical power of some analyses, some co-variates were excluded and some categorical variables were pooled where it was evident that their inclusion did not contribute to the explanatory power of the analysis. Inclusion of second records from a flock did not affect the outcome of analyses, so we included all vigilance allocations and FIDs recorded. As starting distance had a strong influence on FID (see Blumstein 2003), we used adjusted FID (FIDadj), the residuals from an ordinary least squares regression of FID on starting distance, in analyses of tolerance of the doves of an approaching observer. Chi square contingency tests were used to examine species differences in mode and distance of retreat from an approaching observer.

    Due to the FID and vigilance data's non-linearity and the complexity of the behavioural interactions, we used non-linear Conditional Inference Tree and Random Forests models to characterise which variables and co-variates explained most of the variation in FIDadj and the time allocation to vigilance. Conditional Inference Tree models use non-biased splitting and are robust to co-linearity of the variables (Zuur et al. 2007). They progressively split the data into sub-groups with the smallest within-group variation and the largest between-group variation in the tree. Random Forests models are collections of categorical and regression tree analyses (Liaw and Wiener 2002) which can indicate the relative explanatory power of various predictor variables to explain variation in a response variable (Breiman 2001).

    Mean actual FIDs (m) of the three species were: Rock Dove 5.3±0.4, Spotted Dove 14±0.7 and Crested Pigeon 13.4±0.6. Conditional Inference Tree analysis (Figure 1) indicated that FIDadj was significantly shorter in the Rock Dove than in the other two species, which did not differ in approachability. In Rock Doves, FIDadj was also significantly shorter where estimated human population density was higher. Random Forests analysis indicated that for the three species considered collectively, species identity had the strongest proportional influence on FIDadj (76%), with estimated human population density (14%), habitat type and refuge type (each~7%) exerting much smaller influences. No other measured co-variate had any effect on FIDadj.

    Figure  1.  Conditional Inference Tree model for adjusted Flight Initiation Distance in three urban dove species.
    CP=Crested Pigeon, SD=Spotted Dove and RD=Rock Dove. Modelling conducted on arcsine square-root transformed data. Summary statistics for Nodes 2, 4 and 5 on a scale from -15 to +15. Q1 and Q3 are first and third quartiles, respectively. Minimum and maximum values do not include outliers.

    The three species varied in their mode of retreat from the approaching observer (χ2 (4)=102.5, P < 0.001) (Figure 2). Inspection of the standardized residuals (Table 1) indicates that Rock Doves walked away more and flew away less than expected, Crested Pigeons ran away more and flew away less than expected, and Spotted Doves flew away more but walked and ran away less than expected. The species also varied in the distance that they retreated from the observer (χ2 (4)=86.2, P < 0.001) (Figure 2). The standardized residuals (Table 1) indicate that Rock Doves fled≤5 m more and > 10 m less than expected, Spotted Doves fled > 10 m more and≤5 m less than expected and Crested Pigeons retreated 5.1-10 m more and > 10 m less than expected.

    Figure  2.  Retreat mode and distance of three urban dove species to an approaching observer.
    Black columns=Rock Dove, white columns=Spotted Dove and grey columns=Crested Pigeon. n=234.
    Table  1.  Standardised residuals for chi square analyses of retreat mode and distance of urban doves
    Species
    Retreat mode Crested Pigeon Rock Dove Spotted Dove
    Walk -0.8 +3.69 -3.21
    Run +4.2 -1.14 -2.91
    Fly -2.85 -2.82 +5.87
    Retreat distance (m)
    ≤ 5 +0.35 +3.49 -4.04
    5.1-10 +2.44 -1.63 -0.74
    > 10 -2.5 -3.05 +5.74
    Residuals > 2 in bold.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Rock Doves' ground-foraging occurred fairly equally in the quite open habitat of public parks (45.7% of observations) and the more visually obstructed habitat of streetscapes (43.3%), but much less on very open sports fields (11.0%). Spotted Doves' ground-foraging occurred mainly in streetscapes (78.5% of observations) and much less in parks (13.5%) and on sports fields (8.0%). Ground-foraging of Crested Pigeons took place mostly on sports fields (63.0% of observations), far less in more visually obstructed streetscapes (23.6%) and least in parks (13.4%).

    Conditional Inference Tree analysis indicated that the vigilance time allocation of ground-foraging Rock Doves (mean 6.0±0.9%, n=87) was significantly smaller than that of either Spotted Doves (15.4±1.5%, n=67) or Crested Pigeons (15.5±1.9%, n=87), whose allocations were similar (Figures 3 and 4). Vigilance allocation was also greater when flock size was≤5 (20.6±2% of time) than when it was > 5 individuals (10.7±1%) in Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons, but was unaffected by flock size in Rock Doves (Figure 4). Random Forests analysis indicated that for the three species considered collectively, species (40.0%) and flock size (25.0%) had the greatest proportional influences on vigilance allocation, with the presence of con-specifics also exerting about a 15% proportional influence. All other co-variates had a proportional influence≤5%.

    Figure  3.  Mean (± SE) proportional time spent in vigilance by three urban dove species.
    RD=Rock Dove, SD=Spotted Dove and CP=Crested Pigeon.
    Figure  4.  Conditional Inference Tree model for proportional time allocation to vigilance in three urban dove species.
    CP=Crested Pigeon, SD=Spotted Dove and RD=Rock Dove. Modelling conducted on arcsine square-root transformed data. Summary statistics for Nodes 3, 4 and 5 on a scale from 0 to 10. Q1 and Q3 are first and third quartiles, respectively. Minimum and maximum values do not include outliers.

    The Rock Dove had the shortest FIDadj to an approaching observer, the slowest dominant mode of retreat (walking) and the shortest principal retreat distance. However, contrary to our prediction, the Crested Pigeon had a similar FIDadj and vigilance time allocation to the Spotted Dove, and a slower dominant mode of retreat and shorter average retreat distance than the latter species.

    Among bird species, longer FIDs are variously associated with greater body size, older age at first reproduction, omnivory and carnivory, cooperative breeding and habitat 'openness' (Blumstein 2006). Among urban birds, species that have genetically adapted to cities for many generations have shorter FIDs than recent urban colonists (Møller 2008). However, for the species in the present study, none of these traits explained the observed disparities in FID. The largest species, the Rock Dove (34.5 cm mean total length), actually had a significantly smaller FIDadj than the other species, and the smallest species, the Spotted Dove (30.5 cm), had a similar FIDadj to the slightly larger Crested Pigeon (32.5 cm). All three related species are granivores, differing substantially in their diet only in the volume of human food waste consumed in the urban environment (Higgins and Davies 1996; Mulhall and Lill 2011). None of them has been recorded breeding cooperatively to any significant extent (Ford et al. 1988).

    Møller et al. (2014) showed that, particularly in Europe, farmland birds tended to have longer FIDs than those in other habitats. This may be related to both the declining bird population densities and the relative lack of cover (and hence birds' greater vulnerability to predation) in agricultural land. Rock Doves, which had the shortest mean FIDadj, foraged mainly and equally in fairly open public parks and more visually obstructed streetscapes. However, Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons, which had a similar mean FIDadj, contrasted in foraging habitat 'openness', the former mostly using streetscapes with many visual obstructions and the latter very open sports fields. The Rock Dove, which has probably been present in Melbourne longest (possibly for > 200 years), was the least wary of humans. However, whilst the Spotted Dove, which has been resident in the city probably for c. 150 years, was more wary of human proximity than the Rock Dove, it was also more wary than the very recent urban colonizer, the Crested Pigeon. Our comparison of three dove species in the urban environment substantially controlled for purely phylogenetic influences on tolerance of disturbance by humans. However, it must be conceded that it is probably less likely to reveal the particular influences on FID of the type listed above than a phylogenetically broader comparison.

    Natural selection for tolerance of human proximity may have occurred in urban Rock and Spotted Doves, but it cannot plausibly explain the fact that Crested Pigeons were more tolerant than Spotted Doves. It is possible that the observed pattern of tolerance occurred because Crested Pigeons as a species, or the subset of urban founder Crested Pigeons, were bolder than Spotted Doves even after generations of selection for tolerance in the latter species. However, it seems more likely that behavioural flexibility was involved in the greater tolerance exhibited by Crested Pigeons. Such greater flexibility could theoretically be related to relative brain size, which is significantly positively correlated with urban colonizing ability in some birds (Malakov et al. 2011), but was not compared among the present study species. However, Guay et al. (2013) have shown that at least in shorebirds brain volume is not correlated with FID. FID being shorter where estimated human population density was higher in the present investigation could be attributable to either habituation or differential settlement by individuals varying inherently in tolerance of disturbance.

    Anti-predator vigilance is another variable reflecting tolerance of human and predator proximity (Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder 2003). Greater investment in vigilance can increase the distance at which approaching predators and people are detected and/or the probability of detecting them. This enhanced detection ability gives the bird more flexibility to adaptively increase or decrease FID, depending on other relevant variables (e.g. perceived predation risk, Lima and Dill 1990; body mass, Creswell 1995). Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder (2003) showed that Spot-winged Pigeons (Columba maculosa) had a greater proportional time allocation to vigilance and a longer mean FID than co-habiting, smaller Eared Doves (Zenaida auriculata). Presumably for Spot-winged Doves the optimal adaptive response if predators are detected at a great distance is, for whatever reason, to flee early. In our investigation, Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons seemed to have this greater flexibility to adaptively adjust their FID. They had a significantly greater proportional vigilance allocation than Rock Doves and hence probably a superior ability to detect approaching predators and people, and they retreated from an approaching human significantly earlier than Rock Doves. As Rock Doves in Melbourne are directly fed by humans much more than the other two species, they may have down-regulated anti-predator surveillance as the risk posed by people is perceived as being smaller (Lima and Bednekoff 1999).

    The similar vigilance budgets of Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons are intriguing, because the former forages in a more visually obstructed habitat than the latter and consequently might be expected to spend more time being vigilant (Lazarus and Symonds 1992). However, Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder (2003) found that proportional visual scanning time actually decreased as visually obstructive tree cover increased for urban Spot-winged Pigeons and Eared Doves, so the greater level of visual obstruction in streetscapes might be contributing to the similarity in vigilance budgets of Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons. Conceivably, at high levels of visual obstruction vigilance may become relatively ineffective and be down-regulated. The observation that vigilance effort of Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons was less in larger flocks accords with many, but certainly not all, findings for other bird species (Beauchamp 2008), but it is intriguing that the highly gregarious Rock Doves did not exhibit the same relationship. Currently we know too little about natural predation on these three urban doves to factor it into the vigilance formula.

    Whilst it is valid and informative to compare bird species' surveillance for predators by measuring relative vigilance time allocations, two qualifications are pertinent. First, some species' visual fields probably permit surveillance for predators during head-down foraging (Fernández-Juricic et al. 2008), although this may be less of an issue when, as here, closely-related species (which probably have similar visual fields) are being compared. Second, even when species' proportional vigilance time allocations are similar, differences in scanning regime structure may nonetheless result in disparate predator detection efficacies (Fernández-Juricic 2012). These aspects of vigilance require examination in the doves investigated here.

    The Rock Dove, probably the earliest of the study species to have colonised Melbourne, was the most tolerant of human disturbance, having the shortest FID and smallest proportional vigilance effort. However, the Crested Pigeon, the most recent of the three species to have colonised the city, was as, or more, tolerant of disturbance by humans as the second-longest resident, the Spotted Dove. We conclude that:

    (1) genetic selection cannot entirely explain the pattern of tolerance of disturbance by humans observed among the study species and behavioural flexibility appears likely to be involved

    (2) more generally, length of residency in a city is not an infallible guide to a species' relative level of tolerance of disturbance by humans

    (3) logically, we might predict that FID and vigilance effort would be positively correlated in bird species communities and Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder (2003) provide some supporting evidence for such a correlation. Our observations were also consistent with this prediction. Vigilance effort therefore has potential as a metric of tolerance of disturbance in urban birds. However, we still need much more information on the identity and abundance of natural predators in cities to fully understand the implications of interspecific variation in vigilance effort.

    Our study was essentially a 'snapshot' of the current tolerance of disturbance by humans of three dove species in the urban environment. Longitudinal studies of recent urban colonists and their exurban conspecifics would be very demanding, but would allow us to determine if and how tolerance of such disturbance alters through the various stages of urban colonization, from initial settlement to widespread establishment. This would help in defining the relative roles of genetic adaptation and behavioural flexibility in adjusting to disturbance by humans and identifying the specific influences on the acquisition of tolerance to such disturbance.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    JG collected all the field data and conducted much of the data analysis. AL was involved in project conception and management, some data analysis and wrote the manuscript. JB was involved in project conception and management. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    We thank Christopher P. Johnstone for statistical assistance. The study was approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the School of Biological Sciences, Monash University.

  • Albrechtová J, Albrecht T, Baird SJE, Macholán M, Rudolfsen G, Munclinger P, Tucker PK, Piálek J. Sperm-related phenotypes implicated in both maintenance and breakdown of a natural species barrier in the house mouse. Proc R Soc B. 2012;279:4803-10.
    Bennison C, Hemmings N, Slate J, Birkhead T. Long sperm fertilize more eggs in a bird. Proc R Soc B. 2015;. doi: .
    Birkhead TR, Martinez JG, Burke T, Froman DP. Sperm mobility determines the outcome of sperm competition in the domestic fowl. Proc R Soc B. 1999;266:1759-64.
    Birkhead TR, Pellatt EJ, Brekke P, Yeates R, Castillo-Juarez H. Genetic effects on sperm design in the zebra finch. Nature. 2005;434:383-7.
    Birkhead TR, Immler S, Pellatt EJ, Freckleton R. Unusual sperm morphology in the eurasian bullfinch (Pyrrhula pyrrhula). Auk. 2006;123:383-92.
    Bowman DMJS, Brown GK, Braby MF, Brown JR, Cook LG, Crisp MD, Ford F, Haberle S, Hughes J, Isagi Y, Joseph L, McBride J, Nelson G, Ladiges PY. Biogeography of the Australian monsoon tropics. J Biogeog. 2010;37:201-16.
    Briskie JV, Montgomerie R. Patterns of sperm storage in relation to sperm competition in passerine birds. Condor. 1993;95:442-54.
    Briskie JV, Montgomerie R, Birkhead TR. The evolution of sperm size in birds. Evolution. 1997;51:937-45.
    Calhim S, Immler S, Birkhead TR. Postcopulatory sexual selection is associated with reduced variation in sperm morphology. PLoS One. 2007;2:e143.
    Cramer ERA, Laskemoen T, Stensrud E, Rowe M, Haas F, Lifjeld JT, Saetre G-P, Johnsen A. Morphology-function relationships and repeatability in the sperm of Passersparrows. J Morphol. 2015;276:370-7.
    Cruz-Orive LM, Weibel ER. Recent stereological methods for cell biology: a brief survey. Am J Physiol. 1990;258:148-56.
    De Deckker P. Late quaternary cyclic aridity in tropical Australia. Palaeogeogr Palaeocl. 2001;170:1-9.
    Dean MD, Nachman MW. Faster fertilization rate in conspecific versus heterospecific matings in house mice. Evolution. 2009;63:20-8.
    Denk AG, Holzmann A, Peters A, Vermeirssen ELM, Kempenaers B. Paternity in mallards: effects of sperm quality and female sperm selection for inbreeding avoidance. Behav Ecol. 2005;16:825-33.
    Fitzpatrick JL, Baer B. Polyandry reduces sperm length variation in social insects. Evolution. 2011;65:3006-12.
    Fitzpatrick JL, Lüpold S. Sexual selection and the evolution of sperm quality. Mol Hum Reprod. 2014;20:1180-9.
    Fitzpatrick JL, Montgomerie R, Desjardins JK, Stiver KA, Kolm N, Balshine S. Female promiscuity promotes the evolution of faster sperm in cichlid fishes. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:1128-32.
    Harcourt AH, Purvis A, Liles L. Sperm competition: mating system, not breeding season, affects testes size of primates. Funct Ecol. 1995;9:468-76.
    Helfenstein F, Podevin M, Richner H. Sperm morphology, swimming velocity, and longevity in the house sparrowPasser domesticus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;64:557-65.
    Hettyey A, Roberts JD. Sperm traits of the quacking frog, Crinia georgiana:Intra- and interpopulation variation in a species with a high risk of sperm competition. Behav EcolSociobiol. 2006;59:389-96.
    Higgins PJ, Peter JM, Cowling SJ. Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic Birds. Boatbill to Starlings (vol. 7). Melbourne; Oxford University Press; 2006.
    Hogner S, Laskemoen T, Lifjeld JT, Pavel V, Chutný B, García J, Eybert M-C, Matsyna E, Johnsen A. Rapid sperm evolution in the bluethroat (Luscinia svecica) subspecies complex. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2013;67:1205-17.
    Hosken DJ. Sperm competition in bats. Proc R Soc B. 1997;264:385-92.
    Hosken DJ, Garner TWJ, Blanckenhorn WU. Asymmetry, testis and sperm size in Yellow Dung Flies. Funct Ecol. 2003;17:231-6.
    Howard DJ, Palumbi SR, Birge LM, Manier MK. Sperm and speciation. In: Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, Pitnick S, editors. Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2009.
    Humphries S, Evans JP, Simmons LW. Sperm competition: linking form to function. BMC Evol Biol. 2008;8:319.
    Immler S, Calhim S, Birkhead TR. Increased postcopulatory sexual selection reduces the intra-male variation in sperm design. Evolution. 2008;62:1538-43.
    Immler S, Pitnick S, Parker GA, Durrant KL, Lüpold S, Calhim S, Birkhead TR. Resolving variation in the reproductive tradeoff between sperm size and number. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:5325-30.
    Jamieson BGM. Avian spermatozoa: structure and phylogeny. In: Jamieson BGM, editor. Reproductive biology and phylogeny of birds. Enfield, New Hampshire: Science Publisher; 2007.
    Jennings WB, Edwards SV. Speciational history of Australian grass finches (Poephila) inferred from thirty gene trees. Evolution. 2005;59:2033-47.
    Kleven O, Laskemoen T, Fossøy F, Robertson RJ, Lifjeld JT. Intraspecific variation in sperm length is negatively related to sperm competition in passerine birds. Evolution. 2007;62:494-9.
    Kleven O, Fossøy F, Laskemoen T, Robertson RJ, Rudolfsen G, Lifjeld JT. Comparative evidence for the evolution of sperm swimming speed by sperm competition and female sperm storage duration in passerine birds. Evolution. 2009;63:2466-73.
    Landry C, Geyer LB, Arakaki Y, Uehara T, Palumbi SR. Recent speciation in the Indo-West Pacific: rapid evolution of gamete recognition and sperm morphology in cryptic species of sea urchin. Proc R Soc B. 2003;270:1839-47.
    Laskemoen T, Kleven O, Fossøy F, Lifjeld JT. Intraspecific variation in sperm lengthin two passerine species, the Bluethroat Luscinia svecica and the Willow Warbler Phylloscopus trochilus. Ornis Fennica. 2007;84:131-9.
    Laskemoen T, Kleven O, Fossøy F, Robertson RJ, Rudolfsen G, Lifjeld JT. Sperm quantity and quality effects on fertilization success in a highly promiscuous passerine, the tree swallowTachycineta bicolor. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;64:1473-84.
    Laskemoen T, Albrecht T, Bonisoli-Alquati A, Cepak J, Lope F, Hermosell IG, Johannessen LE, Kleven O, Marzal A, Mousseau TA, Møller AP, Robertson RJ, Rudolfsen G, Saino N, Vortman Y, Lifjeld JT. Variation in sperm morphometry and sperm competition among barn swallow (Hirundo rustica) populations. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2013;67:301-9.
    Lifjeld JT, Laskemoen T, Kleven O, Albrecht T, Robertson RJ. Sperm length variation as a predictor of extrapair paternity in passerine birds. PLoS One. 2010;5:e13456.
    Lifjeld JT, Hoenen A, Johannessen LE, Laskemoen T, Lopes RJ, Rodrigues P, Rowe M. The Azores bullfinch (Pyrrhula murina) has the same unusual and size-variable sperm morphology as the Eurasian bullfinch (Pyrrhula pyrrhula). Biol J Linn Soc. 2013;108:677-87.
    Lüpold S. Ejaculate quality and constraints in relation to sperm competition levels among eutherian mammals. Evolution. 2013;67:3052-60.
    Lüpold S, Calhim S, Immler S, Birkhead TR. Sperm morphology and sperm velocity in passerine birds. Proc R Soc B. 2009;276:1175-81.
    Lüpold S, Westneat DF, Birkhead TR. Geographical variation in sperm morphology in the red-winged blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus). Evol Ecol. 2011;25:373-90.
    Lüpold S, Manier MK, Berben KS, Smith KJ, Daley BD, Buckley SH, Belote JM, Pitnick S. How multivariate ejaculate traits determine competitive fertilization success in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol. 2012;22:1667-72.
    Manier MK, Palumbi SR. Intraspecific divergence in sperm morphology of the green sea urchin, Strongylocentrotus droebachlensis: implications for selection in broadcast spawners. BMC Evol Biol. 2008;8:283.
    Manier MK, Belote JM, Berben KS, Lüpold S, Ala-Honkola O, Collins WF, Pitnick S. Rapid diversification of sperm precedence traits and processes among three sibling Drosophila species. Evolution. 2013a;67:2348-62.
    Manier MK, Lüpold S, Belote JM, Starmer WT, Berben KS, Ala-Honkola O, Collins WF, Pitnick S. Postcopulatory sexual selection generates speciation phenotypes in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2013b;23:1853-62.
    Minoretti N, Baur B. Among- and within-population variation in sperm quality in the simultaneously hermaphroditic land snail Arianta arbustorum. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:270-80.
    Møller AP. Sperm competition, sperm depletion, paternal care, and relative testis size in birds. Am Nat. 1991;137:882-906.
    Mossman J, Slate J, Humphries S, Birkhead TR. Sperm morphology and velocity are genetically codetermined in the zebra finch. Evolution. 2009;63:2730-7.
    Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Core Team. nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-118. 2014. ].
    Pitnick S, Miller GT, Schneider K, Markow TA. Ejaculate-female coevolution in Drosophila mojavensis. Proc R Soc B. 2003;270:1507-12.
    Pitnick S, Hosken DJ, Birkhead TR. Sperm morphological diversity. In: Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, Pitnick S, editors. Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2009.
    Pizzari T, Parker GA (2009) Sperm competition and sperm phenotype. In Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, Pitnick S, editors. Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2009.
    Pizzari T, Worley K, Burke T, Froman DP. Sperm competition dynamics: ejaculate fertilising efficiency changes differentially with time. BMC Evol Biol. 2008;8:332-8.
    R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2013. .
    Rollins LA, Svedin N, Pryke SR, Griffith SC. The role of the Ord Arid Intrusion in the historical and contemporary genetic division of long-tailed finch subspecies in northern Australia. Ecol Evol. 2012;2:1208-19.
    Rowe M, Pruett-Jones S. Sperm competition selects for sperm quantity and quality in the Australian Maluridae. PLoS One. 2011;6:e15720.
    Rowe M, Albrecht T, Cramer ERA, Johnsen A, Laskemoen T, Weir JT, Lifjeld JT. Postcopulatory sexual selection is associated with accelerated evolution of sperm morphology. Evolution. 2015;69:1044-52.
    Schmoll T, Kleven O. Sperm dimensions differ between two Coal Tit Periparus ater populations. J Ornithol. 2010;152:515-20.
    Schodde R, Mason IJ. The directory of australian birds. Collingwood: CSIRO Publishing; 1999.
    Simmons LW, Fitzpatrick JL. Sperm wars and the evolution of male fertility. Reproduction. 2012;144:519-34.
    Simmons LW, Moore AJ. Evolutionary quantitative genetics of sperm. In: Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, Pitnick S, editors. Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2009.
    Smith CC. Opposing effects of sperm viability and velocity on the outcome of sperm competition. Behav Ecol. 2012;23:820-6.
    Snook RR. Sperm in competition: not playing by the numbers. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20:46-53.
    Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ. Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research. San Francisco: Freeman; 1995.
    Stockley P, Gage MJG, Parker GA, Møller AP. Sperm competition in fishes: the evolution of testis size and ejaculate characteristics. Am Nat. 1997;149:933-54.
    Swanson WJ, Vacquier VD. The rapid evolution of reproductive proteins. Nat Rev Genet. 2002;3:137-44.
    Töpfer T, Haring E, Birkhead TR, Lopes RJ, Severinghaus L, Martens J, Påckert M. A molecular phylogeny of bullfinches Pyrrhula Brisson, 1760 (Aves: Fringillidae). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2011;58:271-82.
    Tourmente M, Gomendio M, Roldan ERS. Sperm competition and the evolution of sperm design in mammals. BMC Evol Biol. 2011a;11:12.
    Tourmente M, Gomendio M, Roldan ERS. Mass-specific metabolic rate and sperm competition determine sperm size in marsupial mammals. PLoSone. 2011b;6:e21244.
    Van Rooij EP, Griffith SC. No evidence of assortative mating on the basis of putative ornamental traits in Long-tailed Finches Poephila acuticauda. Ibis. 2012;154:444-51.
    Varea-Sánchez M, Gómez Montoto L, Tourmente M, Roldan ERS. Postcopulatory sexual selection results in spermatozoa with more uniform head and flagellum sizes in rodents. PLoS One. 2014;9:e108148.
    Veen T, Borge T, Griffith SC, Saetre G-P, Bures S, Gustafsson L, Sheldon BC. Hybridization and adaptive mate choice in flycatchers. Nature. 2001;411:45-50.
    Wolfson A. The cloacal protuberance—a means for determining breeding condition in live male passerines. Bird Banding. 1952;23:159-65.
    Zann R. Variation in the songs of three species of Estrildine grassfinches. Emu. 1976;76:97-108.
    Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York: Springer; 2009.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(12)

    1. Takeshi Honda, Hironori Ueda. Why Mammals do Not Damage Entire Farmlands Like Insect Pests Do? A Review from a Behavioral Perspective. Mammal Study, 2023, 48(2) DOI:10.3106/ms2022-0054
    2. Luqin Yin, Cheng Wang, Wenjing Han, et al. Birds’ Flight Initiation Distance in Residential Areas of Beijing Are Lower than in Pristine Environments: Implications for the Conservation of Urban Bird Diversity. Sustainability, 2023, 15(6): 4994. DOI:10.3390/su15064994
    3. Melissa Ardila-Villamizar, Gustavo Alarcón-Nieto, Adriana A. Maldonado-Chaparro. Fear in urban landscapes: conspecific flock size drives escape decisions in tropical birds. Royal Society Open Science, 2022, 9(11) DOI:10.1098/rsos.221344
    4. Swaroop Patankar, Ravi Jambhekar, Kulbhushansingh Ramesh Suryawanshi, et al. Which Traits Influence Bird Survival in the City? A Review. Land, 2021, 10(2): 92. DOI:10.3390/land10020092
    5. Takeshi Honda. Geographical personality gradient in herbivorous animals: Implications for selective culling to reduce crop damage. Ecological Research, 2021, 36(1): 145. DOI:10.1111/1440-1703.12186
    6. Takeshi Honda, Yohichi Kubota, Yutaka Ishizawa. Ungulates-exclusion grates as an adjoining facility to crop damage prevention fences. European Journal of Wildlife Research, 2020, 66(1) DOI:10.1007/s10344-020-1362-7
    7. Daniel T. Blumstein. What chasing birds can teach us about predation risk effects: past insights and future directions. Journal of Ornithology, 2019, 160(2): 587. DOI:10.1007/s10336-019-01634-1
    8. Jenna L. Van Donselaar, Jenna L. Atma, Zachary A. Kruyf, et al. Urbanization alters fear behavior in black-capped chickadees. Urban Ecosystems, 2018, 21(6): 1043. DOI:10.1007/s11252-018-0783-5
    9. Javier delBarco-Trillo. Shyer and larger bird species show more reduced fear of humans when living in urban environments. Biology Letters, 2018, 14(4): 20170730. DOI:10.1098/rsbl.2017.0730
    10. T. Honda, H. Iijima, J. Tsuboi, et al. A review of urban wildlife management from the animal personality perspective: The case of urban deer. Science of The Total Environment, 2018, 644: 576. DOI:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.06.335
    11. Matthew R. E. Symonds, Michael A. Weston, Wouter F. D. van Dongen, et al. Time Since Urbanization but Not Encephalisation Is Associated with Increased Tolerance of Human Proximity in Birds. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2016, 4 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2016.00117
    12. Bao-Sen Shieh, Shih-Hsiung Liang, Yuh-Wen Chiu, et al. Interspecific comparison of traffic noise effects on dove coo transmission in urban environments. Scientific Reports, 2016, 6(1) DOI:10.1038/srep32519

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(2)  /  Tables(2)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (234) PDF downloads (18) Cited by(12)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return