Anders Pape Møller, Johannes Erritzøe. 2015: Brain size and urbanization in birds. Avian Research, 6(1): 8. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0017-y
Citation: Anders Pape Møller, Johannes Erritzøe. 2015: Brain size and urbanization in birds. Avian Research, 6(1): 8. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0017-y

Brain size and urbanization in birds

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Anders Pape Møller, anders.moller@u-psud.fr

  • Received Date: 07 Nov 2014
  • Accepted Date: 14 Mar 2015
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 14 May 2015
  • Background 

    Brain size may affect the probability of invasion of urban habitats if a relatively larger brain entails superior ability to adapt to novel environments. However, once urbanized urban environments may provide poor quality food that has negative consequences for normal brain development resulting in an excess of individuals with small brains.

    Methods 

    Here we analyze the independent effects of mean, standard deviation and skewness in brain mass for invasion of urban habitats by 108 species of birds using phylogenetic multiple regression analyses weighted by sample size.

    Results 

    There was no significant difference in mean brain mass between urbanized and non-urbanized species or between urban and rural populations of the same species, and mean brain mass was not significantly correlated with time since urbanization. Bird species that became urbanized had a greater standard deviation in brain mass than non-urbanized species, and the standard deviation in brain mass increased with time since urbanization. Brain mass was significantly left skewed in species that remained rural, while there was no significant skew in urbanized species. The degree of left skew was greater in urban than in rural populations of the same species, and successfully urbanized species decreased the degree of left skew with time since urbanization. This is consistent with the hypothesis that sub-optimal brain development was more common in rural habitats resulting in disproportionately many individuals with very small brains.

    Conclusions 

    These findings do not support the hypothesis that large brains promote urbanization, but suggest that skewness has played a role in the initial invasion of urban habitats, and that variance and skew in brain mass have increased as species have become urbanized.

  • The well-known arms race Between parasitic cuckoos and their hosts has long been a model system for the study of coevolution and microevolution (Davies, 2000; Soler, 2014). Approximately 2300 years ago, Aristotle (382-322 BC) wrote that "it (i.e., the Common Cuckoo Cuculus canorus) lays its eggs in the nest of smaller birds after devouring these birds's eggs" (Peck 1970). Many years later, the 'father of vaccination', Edward Jenner (1788), observed the ejection behavior of cuckoo chicks and published his finding, on the basis of which he came to be elected a Fellow of the Royal Society. Afterwards Darwin (1859) proposed the first explanation for how the parasitic behavior of cuckoos could have evolved by natural selection. Cuckoos exploit their hosts by transferring parental care to the host and this parasitism is undoubtedly costly for the hosts of the cuckoo (Rothstein and Robinson 1998; Davies 2000; Soler 2014). Furthermore, cuckoo parasitism destroys or severely reduces the reproductive success of its hosts (Davies 2011). This special behavior has provoked evolution of anti-parasitic defences in hosts, mainly involving a specific aggressive response towards cuckoos, a recognition of cuckoo eggs or chicks, a counter-adaptation against cuckoo trickery (e.g., egg mimicry) or fine tuning of parasitic adaptations (e.g., rapid egg-laying) (Dawkins and Krebs 1979; Davies 2011). Although this coevolutionary interaction between cuckoos and their hosts has been studied for a long time and a series of theories and hypotheses have been proposed, such as evolutionary lag, evolutionary equilibrium, strong eggshell and rapid laying of eggs by parasites, interaction between egg mimicry and egg recognition, host imprinting, host shift and chick recognition (Brooke and Davies 1988; Moksnes et al. 1991; Davies 2000; Røskaft et al. 2002; Langmore et al. 2003; Kilner 2006; Yang et al. 2013a), so far some puzzles have remained unsolved. For example, the question of whether cuckoos parasitize their hosts by laying eggs randomly or matching the egg morphs in host nests is one of the mysteries of the cuckoo problem (Davies 2000, Antonov et al. 2012).

    To date four empirical papers have been published to test this mystery (Avilés et al. 2006, Cherry et al. 2007, Antonov et al. 2012, Honza et al. 2014; Table 1), but only one showed that cuckoos lay eggs randomly in the nests of their hosts (Antonov et al. 2012). Scientists tend to believe that cuckoos lay eggs matching the appearance of host eggs due to selection caused by a high egg recognition ability of the hosts (Avilés et al. 2006; Cherry et al. 2007; Honza et al. 2014).

    Table  1.  Summary of previous studies of egg matching by cuckoos and current theories violated by the findings
    Former studies Data source Method Main findings Theories violated by the findings Costs for the cuckoos
    Avilés et al. (2006) Museum and field data comparing cuckoo egg matching between parasitized and non-parasitized nests cuckoos laid matching eggs secretive behavior and rapid egg-laying of cuckoos; host imprinting and host selection by cuckoos increase the risk of detection by hosts; loss of time searching for nests and monitoring host behavior; mis-imprinting in host selection
    Cherry et al. (2007) Field data cuckoos laid matching eggs
    Honza et al.(2014) Field data cuckoos laid matching eggs
    Antonov et al. (2012) Field data cuckoo laid eggs randomly - waste of poor-matching eggs in nests when hosts are good rejecters
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    At first, we briefly review previous empirical studies which have examined this mystery, provide examples of unmatched cuckoo eggs in host nests and key life history traits of cuckoos, e.g. their secretive behavior and rapid egg-laying and link them to cuckoo egg laying behavior. We then develop a conceptual model to demonstrate why cuckoos should utilize their hosts by laying eggs randomly rather than matching the appearance of host eggs. We opted for coevolution between Common Cuckoos and their parrotbill hosts (Paradoxornis alphonsianus), both of which have evolved polymorphic eggs (mainly blue and white) (Yang et al. 2010, 2013b), as an example for the model and follow with a discussion of this issue. In the end, we propose an empirical test that can provide direct evidence concerning the egg-laying properties of female cuckoos.

    It has been supposed that cuckoo nestlings imprint on their foster parents and return to parasitize them as adults (Lack 1968; Davies 2000); however, laying a matching egg is not necessary. According to previous studies, cuckoos lay non-mimetic eggs in nests of many regular hosts (Payne 2005; Lee 2008; Yang et al. 2012a, 2012b; Lowther 2013). For example, Lee (2008) found that the Common Cuckoo laid 52.6% of unmatched cuckoo eggs in the nests of the Vinous-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis webbianus) that lays polymorphic eggs. This percentage is a considerable underestimation because the hosts rejected 82.6% of poorly-matching eggs and 16.7% of well-matching eggs (Lee 2008) and hence many unmatched cuckoo eggs should have been rejected before their detection by observers. Furthermore, cuckoos laid 100% non-matching eggs in Dunnock (Prunella modularis) nests (Davies and Brooke 1989). Since cuckoos do not experience the responses to their eggs by hosts as dunnocks do, nor recognize unmatched eggs (i.e., accept or reject cuckoo eggs), they should not lay non-mimetic eggs in dunnock nests, if cuckoos were to lay eggs based on their own egg appearance.

    The first tentative study considering cuckoo-host egg matching was by Avilés et al. (2006), which is a summary of the temporal changes in the degree of matching between Common Cuckoo and host (Acrocephalus scirpaceus) eggs, over a period of 24 consecutive years. They found that ultraviolet-brownness of cuckoo eggs was similar to that of host eggs at parasitized nests but differed from that of host eggs at non-parasitized nests (Avilés et al. 2006). Subsequently, three short-term studies investigated the degree of cuckoo-host egg matching between parasitized and non-parasitized nests (Cherry et al. 2007; Antonov et al. 2012; Honza et al. 2014). Cherry et al. (2007) tested this hypothesis in the Great Reed Warbler (A. arundinaceus), while Antonov et al. (2012) conducted an experiment with the Mash Warbler (A. palustris). However, these two studies present opposing conclusions. A final study by Honza et al. (2014) of great reed warblers quantified egg color by relying on physiological modeling of avian color vision. They also assessed cuckoo egg matching in host clutches that were suitable for parasitism in terms of timing but remained non-parasitized (Honza et al. 2014). However, multi-parasitized nests were excluded from their study. A total of 19 nests (31%) out of 61 nests were parasitized, while four nests (21%) were double parasitized and hence not included in the analysis (Honza et al. 2014).

    These empirical studies attempted to assign parasitism status correctly in order to avoid the idea that cuckoo eggs in some parasitized nests had been rejected by hosts before their detection. These efforts included marking host eggs in each nest soon after laying (Cherry et al. 2007, Honza et al. 2014) or using nests found during nest building or at early stages of egg laying (Antonov et al. 2012) (Table 1). All the same, the potential risk of undetected parasitism and rejection by hosts still exists, no matter how small. Logically, only real full-time monitoring can completely exclude this bias. So far among these previous studies, Honza et al. (2014) have provided convincing support for solving this problem. However, they have not analyzed mimicry of egg pattern, which cannot be quantified by spectra. Recently new pattern quantification techniques from avian vision were developed (Stoddard and Stevens 2010; Stoddard et al. 2014), which may eliminate this restriction. Furthermore, since Common Cuckoos remove one host egg before laying their own egg (Davies 2000), scientists would be unable to compare the whole clutch of parasitized nests with that of non-parasitized nests, contributing further bias to studies. Such effects may be slight in host species with low intraclutch variation but can be severe in species with high intraclutch variation. To eliminate this problem, the spectra of each host egg should be measured soon after it is laid to avoid omission of any egg removal by cuckoos. Such frequent manipulation will exert considerable disturbance on both hosts and cuckoos, increase the rate of nest desertion of hosts and obstruct cuckoo parasitism, since cuckoos usually lay eggs during the egg-laying period of their hosts (Davies 2000). Moreover, such disturbance will also increase or decrease the risk of predation (Ibáñnz-Álamo et al. 2012). All these potential risks may together affect the results and cause bias. Additionally, none of these studies provide direct evidence of cuckoos choosing to parasitize host nests where egg color and pattern match. The degree of egg matching between cuckoo eggs and those of a host, as detected by humans, should be caused by egg recognition ability of hosts, rather than the selection of matching host eggs by cuckoos (Table 1).

    In order to deceive their hosts successfully, parasitic cuckoos have evolved a variety of tricks, selected for various anti-parasitic defences by hosts (Davies 2011). At first, female cuckoos should behave secretively to gain access to host nests for egg laying to avoid detection by hosts (Payne 1977). Detection, mobbing or attack by hosts are costly for cuckoos. Mobbing or attack by hosts may cause failure of egg-laying, injury and even have lethal consequence for adult cuckoos (Liversidge 1970; Davies 2000, 2011; Røskaft et al. 2002; Krüger 2011). For example, the mobbing by the bulbul (Pycnonotus capensis) makes it difficult for the female Jacobin cuckoo (Clamator jacobinus) to gain access to the host nest, but also difficult to monitor host behavior and hence time her laying correctly. In the end, many cuckoo eggs are laid too late and fail to hatch (Liversidge 1970; Krüger 2011). Furthermore, exposure, when laying eggs, also increases the rejection rate of cuckoo eggs because hosts may enhance their ability to discriminate against foreign eggs from increased risk of parasitism (e.g. Brooke et al. 1998; Stokke et al. 2008). Therefore, female cuckoos have evolved an astonishing ability of rapid egg-laying, i.e., in 7-158 seconds, a strong selection (evolutionary?) option as a consequence of nest defence by hosts (Payne 1977; Davies 2000; Moksnes et al. 2000). Fast egg laying in most obligate interspecific brood parasites is common and may have evolved to minimize host detection, which can elicit host defences and lower the likelihood of successful parasitism (Davies and Brooke Davies and Brooke 1988; Kattan 1997; Langmore et al. 2003; Mermoz and Reboreda 2003). Hosts can increase their defences when detecting parasite activity, which should select for cryptic habits in brood parasites (Moksnes et al. 1991; Bártol et al. 2003; Feeney et al. 2012).

    However, when cuckoos search for host nests and lay eggs matching the appearance of the eggs of their hosts based on their own egg morphs, this will considerably increase the risk of detection by hosts because of high activity during parasitism. For example, a female Common Cuckoo of the parrotbill-specific gentes that lays blue eggs, should parasitize blue clutches of hosts. However, she cannot predict the color of host eggs before the female parrotbill lays them. Although parasitism generally occurs during the laying period, cuckoos spend most of their time monitoring the reproductive activity of their hosts (Davies 2000). Consequently, we can imagine that the blue-egg cuckoo would have to neglect white clutches that she has encountered and keep looking for blue clutches. That allows us to predict the costs for this phenomenon, for (1): this increases the risk of detection by hosts, which may cause subsequent attack or promote egg rejection by hosts (Moksnes et al. 1991; Honza et al. 2002) and (2): it causes loss of time seeking for host nests and monitoring host behavior (Table 1). The negative outcome of the second problem for cuckoos is undoubtedly costly for cuckoos invest time to search for host nests and monitor their breeding behavior within a breeding season (Chance 1940, Davies 2011). If the proportion of blue and white clutches in parrotbills is 1:1, female cuckoos face a probability of only 50% of the host egg color matching that of their own eggs. The real proportion of blue and white clutches in parrotbills is similar to this ratio (Yang et al. 2010).

    Scientists may argue that laying eggs in a host nest randomly is also costly because of the waste of eggs in nests with poorly matching eggs. To compare the costs and benefits, we should consider nest density, habitat distribution of various egg morphs and the ability to recognize different host species in their habitat. We suggest that scientists should use mathematical modeling to quantify the costs of both properties and simulate the outcomes. In addition, egg laying by female cuckoos is so fast (less than 10 seconds, Davies 2000) that it could also prevent cuckoos from watching the host eggs carefully to check for matching status. Furthermore, so far no observation or video recording has shown that a female cuckoo gives up laying an egg in host nests when she finds that the host clutch does not match her egg morph, although some strange behavior of cuckoos, visiting host nests without laying eggs, has been recorded (Moksnes et al. 2000, Honza et al. 2002). Long-time monitoring, secretive approach and rapid egg laying by cuckoos are proven to be widespread and undoubted adaptations, selected by host defences (Rothstein and Robinson 1998; Davies 2000; Soler 2014). Matched egg laying with respect to egg phenotype contradicts these adaptations and thus seems to be maladaptive. One may argue that this inference is not persuasive. In the following we provide further arguments to show that cuckoo egg laying, based on the appearance of their own eggs, is maladaptive for host selection.

    Parasitic cuckoos can lay a variety of egg morphs to utilize different species of hosts. For example, common cuckoos in Europe have been divided into at least 16 host-specific races or gentes based on human visual inspection (Wyllie 1981; Álvarez 1994; Moksnes and Røskaft 1995). The question of how cuckoos maintain these distinct gentes and select hosts remains a puzzle (Honza et al. 2001). Two major hypotheses have been suggested - host imprinting and habitat imprinting (Lack 1968; Lotem 1993; Teuschl et al. 1998). The host imprint hypothesis assumes that a female cuckoo lays the same egg type as her mother and seeks to parasitize the same host species that raised her through imprinting on the characteristics of host parents (Lack 1968, Davies 2000). Therefore, for example, a female cuckoo nestling, raised by parrotbills, should choose to parasitize parrotbill nests when she starts to breed. For the habitat imprinting hypothesis, cuckoo nestlings imprint on the habitats in which they hatched (Moksnes and Røskaft 1995, Teuschl et al. 1998). Another explanation is a mixture of these two hypotheses with a sequence of decisions (Teuschl et al. 1998; Davies 2000). Scientists tend to believe that the most likely is host imprinting as shown for host choice by parasitic finches (Nicolai 1961; Davies 2000), although habitat imprinting may serve as a pre-adaptation for general nest searches by cuckoos (Teuschl et al. 1998, Honza et al. 2002, Vogl et al. 2002).

    Natural selection acts on the phenotypes or the observable characteristics of organisms, which relate to fitness and vary between individuals within populations (Darwin 1859). Therefore, variation in egg phenotypes among individual cuckoos favors those that maximize fitness by utilizing potential new host species, especially when common hosts evolve high rates of egg rejection and cuckoos hence have low reproductive success in commonly parasitized nests compared to nests of novel hosts. For example, common cuckoos have been found to parasitize more than 300 species of hosts, which belong to about 46 families of birds (Lowther 2013).

    We developed a conceptual and straightforward model to illustrate the outcome of potential host selection by cuckoos under two scenarios that reflect random egg laying or phenotypic matching (Figure 1). In this model we hypothesize that a female common cuckoo of the parrotbill gens lays a blue egg (female embryo inside) in a nest of a potential new, naive and suitable host by chance, which lays monomorphic white eggs. If accepted, the female cuckoo egg will hatch, while the host parents will rear the nestling. When this cuckoo chick successfully fledges, she returns to the place of hatching or disperses elsewhere, but chooses to parasitize the new host on which she has imprinted. If cuckoos lay eggs matching those of the hosts, based on their own egg appearance and if this behavior were inherited, this young female cuckoo would lay blue eggs. Thus we can speculate that the reproductive success of this young female cuckoo is zero because she can never find any nest in which the egg color matches her own. Even if imprinting of egg appearance in cuckoos (i.e., knowing their own egg appearance) is acquired through learning rather than inherited, this female cuckoo can only succeed in her first trial of laying for learning, but again fails to find any suitable nest for the rest of her life. By contrast, if cuckoos utilize hosts by laying eggs randomly, they will enjoy greater reproductive success (Figure 2). Therefore, there is a risk of mis-imprinting when cuckoos lay eggs based on the appearance of their own eggs. We also consider additional situations in the model (see Figures 1 and 2 for more details), which are interpreted below.

    Figure  1.  A conceptual model of host selection by cuckoos based on the assumption that cuckoos known their own egg appearance and choose to parasitize hosts by laying eggs matching the appearance of host eggs.
    Figure  2.  A conceptual model of host selection by cuckoos based on the assumption that cuckoos choose to parasitize hosts by laying eggs randomly in host nests.

    Our conceptual model is based on the assumptions that (1): rejectors refer to hosts that reject all non-mimetic eggs and acceptors that accept them, (2): the new host species lays blue eggs, which are similar to the blue cuckoo eggs, but not particularly matching because it has had no coevolutionary history with the cuckoo, hence blue egg rejectors of hosts also reject a proportion of blue cuckoo eggs and (3): the cuckoo chicks imprint on the host species, which raise them.

    According to Figure 1, female cuckoos have a probabilities of p1 to parasitize hosts of rejectors and and q1 of acceptors, where p1 + q1 = 1. Rejectors then can be divided into hosts laying eggs of different appearance, including blue eggs (host A with p2), which are similar to those of the female cuckoos and other egg morphs (host B with q2). However, host B rejects all blue eggs and causes failure of cuckoo parasitism. Host A accepts a proportion (p4) of blue cuckoo eggs, but rejects the others (q4). Only the accepted blue eggs can be incubated by hosts resulting in the cuckoo chicks fledging and choosing to parasitize host A again, with the consequence that the cuckoo chick has a probability of success of q5 and q6 to coevolve with host A. According to the second assumption, this probability (q5 + q6) depends on the egg-matching abilities of female cuckoos. In other words, q5 and q6 decreases with the increasing egg-matching ability of female cuckoos. This is close to zero when cuckoos have a good ability to match the egg appearance of hosts during laying. We included two mechanisms of acquisition of information on the appearance of own eggs in cuckoos in the model. However, even if the ability of learning egg appearance in cuckoos is acquired rather than inherited, female cuckoos can only succeed in their first trials of laying for learning but fail the second time. Similarly, for acceptor hosts laying blue eggs, female cuckoos possess a success rate of q7 and q8, which also decrease with the increase of egg-matching ability by cuckoos. In short, a successful probability is the sum of p5 + p6 + p7 + p8, which is close to zero when cuckoos have a great ability to lay eggs with a high degree of egg matching. In such a situation almost no cuckoo offspring can succeed in utilizing new host species.

    By contrast, the results from the model that cuckoos parasitize new host species by laying eggs randomly are much simpler (Figure 2). Only if the new hosts were rejectors and laid non-blue eggs would this cause failure of cuckoo parasitism.

    We have argued theoretically that laying eggs matching those of the host, violates the key traits in the life history of cuckoos and therefore should not evolve by natural selection.

    First, egg matching behavior is against the secretive behavior of approaching cuckoos near host nests and wastes time by long-term monitoring of host behavior on the part of cuckoos.

    Second, if cuckoos chose hosts with eggs similar to those with their own egg appearance, they would cause extreme constraints on the flexibility of their offspring to accept and adapt to a new host by pushing them into an evolutionary dead-end. Therefore, this would entirely cut off the evolution of exploitation of new hosts. It is likely that the percentage of egg matching between cuckoo and host eggs, as detected by humans, should be caused by the ability of egg recognition on the part of hosts (Davies and Brooke 1989), rather than the selection of matching host eggs by cuckoos.

    Finally, we suggest an empirical method, referred to as an "induced parasitism experiment", that may provide direct evidence to demonstrate how the cuckoo lays its eggs. In such an experiment, scientists establish artificial nests in which eggs of a different appearance are provided for egg laying by cuckoos. In this scenario, female cuckoos can readily monitor the focal nests because of the short distances between nests and lay an egg randomly or match the egg appearance of the eggs in these nests with that of their own eggs. If female cuckoos do not match their eggs with those of their hosts in this scenario of little or no constraints, there is no reason to believe that female cuckoos would be able to achieve a greater level of matching under natural conditions when host nests are considerably more difficult to find. This method should be feasible because Chance (1940) actually carried out a similar trial_ before, collecting cuckoo eggs.

    We conclude by suggesting that cuckoo egg laying by matching host eggs is maladaptive and should not evolve from natural selection.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    WL designed the study. FT, WL and CY discussed and developed the modeling and CY performed all analyses. CY and AP drafted the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the paper.

    This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (nos. 31071938, 31272328 and 31472013 to WL, and 31260514 to CY), the Program for New Century Excellent Talents in University (NCET-13-0761), the Key Project of the Chinese Ministry of Education (no. 212136) and the Program of International S & T Cooperation (KJHZ2013-12) to CY.

  • Almond D, Edlund L, Palmer M (2007) Chernobyl's subclinical legacy: Prenatal exposure to radioactive fallout and school outcomes in Sweden. Natl Bureau Econ Res 13347:1-50
    Ames A (2000) CNS energy metabolism as related to function. J Neurosci 34:42-68
    Armstrong E, Bergeron R (1985) Relative brain size and metabolism in birds. Brain Behav Evol 26:141-153
    Bains JS, Shaw CA (1997) Neurodegenerative disorders in humans: The role of glutathione in oxidative stress-mediated neuronal death. Brain Res Rev 25:335-358
    Baratti M, Cordaro M, Dessi-Fulgheri F, Vannini M, Fratini S (2009) Molecular and ecological characterization of urban populations of the mallard (Anas platyrhynchos) in Italy. Ital J Zool 76:330-339
    Björklund M, Ruiz I, Senar JC (2010) Genetic differentiation in the urban habitat: The great tits (Parus major) of the parks of Barcelona city. Biol J Linn Soc 99:9-19
    Bonaparte KM, Riffle-Yokoi C, Burley NT (2011) Getting a head start: Diet, sub-adult growth, and associative learning in a seed-eating passerine. PLoS One 6(9):e23775
    Carrete M, Tella JL (2011) Inter-individual variability in fear of humans and relative brain size of the species are related to contemporary urban invasion in birds. PLoS One 6(4):e18859
    Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
    Cramp S, Perrins CM (eds) (1977-1994) The Birds of the Western Palearctic. Vols 1-9. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK
    Crile G, Quiring DP (1940) A record of the body weight and certain organ and gland weight of 3690 animals. Ohio J Sci 40:219-259
    Croci S, Butet A, Clergeau P (2008) Does urbanization filter birds on the basis of their biological traits? Condor 110:223-240
    Davis KE (2008) Reweaving the tapestry: a supertree of birds. PhD Thesis. University of Glasgow, Glasgow, Scotland
    Draper NR, Smith H (1981) Applied Regression Analysis, 2nd edn. John Wiley, New York, NY
    Evans KL, Gaston KJ, Frantz AC, Simeoni M, Sharp SP, McGowan A, Dawson DA, Walasz K, Partecke J, Burke T, Hatchwell BJ (2009) Independent colonization of multiple urban centres by a formerly forest specialist bird species. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2403-2410
    Evans KL, Chamberlain DE, Hatchwell BJ, Gregory RD, Gaston KJ (2011) What makes an urban bird? Global Change Biol 17:32-44
    Felsenstein J (1985) Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am Nat 125:1-15
    Fulgione D, Rippa D, Procaccini G, Milone M (2000) Urbanisation and the genetic structure of Passer italiae (Viellot 1817) populations in the south of Italy. Ethol Ecol Evol 12:123-130
    Garamszegi LZ, Møller AP (2010) Effects of sample size and intraspecific variation in phylogenetic comparative studies: A meta-analytic review. Biol Rev 85:797-805
    Gliwicz J, Goszczynski J, Luniak M (1994) Characteristic features of animal populations under synurbanization: The case of the Blackbirds and the striped field mouse. Mem Zool 49:237-244
    Glutz von Blotzheim UN, Bauer KM (eds) (1966-1997) Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas Band. Aula-Verlag, Wiebelsheim, Germany
    Harvey PH, Pagel M (1991) The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK
    Heiervang KS, Mednick S, Sundet K, Rund BR (2010) Effect of low dose ionizing radiation exposure in utero on cognitive function in adolescence. Scand J Psychol 51:210-215
    Hoffman DJ, Heinz GH (1998) Effects of mercury and selenium on glutathione metabolism and oxidative stress in mallard ducks. Environ Toxicol Chem 17:161-166
    Kark S, Iwaniuk A, Schalimtzek A, Banker E (2007) Living in the city: Can anyone become an 'urban exploiter'? J Biogeogr 34:638-651
    Klausnitzer B (1989) Verstädterung von Tieren. Neue Brehm-Bücherei, Wittenberg Lutherstadt, Germany
    Laughlin SB (2001) Energy as a constraint on the coding and processing of sensory information. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:475-480
    Laughlin SB, van Steveninck RRD, Anderson JC (1998) The metabolic cost of neural information. Nat Neurosci 1:36-41
    Maklakov AA, Immler S, Gonzalez-Voyer A, Rönn J, Kolm N (2011) Brains and the city: Big-brained passerine birds succeed in urban environments. Biol Lett 7:730-732
    McClave JT, Sincich T (2003) Statistics, 9th edn. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ
    Mlikovsky J (1990) Brain size in birds: 4. Passeriformes. Acta Soc Zool Bohemoslov 54:27-37
    Møller AP (2008) Flight distance of urban birds, predation and selection for urban life. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:63-75
    Møller AP (2009) Successful city dwellers: A comparative study of the ecological characteristics of urban birds in the Western Palearctic. Oecologia 159:849-858
    Møller AP (2010) Interspecific variation in fear responses predicts urbanization in birds. Behav Ecol 21:365-371
    Møller AP, Nielsen JT (2007) Malaria and risk of predation: A comparative study of birds. Ecology 88:871-881
    Møller AP, Surai PF, Mousseau TA (2005) Antioxidants, radiation and mutation in barn swallows from Chernobyl. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:247-253
    Møller AP, Erritzøe J, Karadas F (2010) Levels of antioxidants in rural and urban birds and their consequences. Oecologia 163:35-45
    Møller AP, Bonisoli-Alquati A, Rudolfsen G, Mousseau TA (2011) Chernobyl birds have smaller brains. PLoS One 6(2):e16862
    Møller AP, Diaz M, Flensted-Jensen E, Grim T, Ibáñez-Álamo JD, Jokimäki J, Mänd R, Marko G, Tryjanowski P (2012) High urban population density of birds reflects their timing of urbanization. Oecologia 170:867-875
    Neter J, Kutner MH, Nachtsheim CJ, Wasserman W (1996) Applied Linear Statistical Models. Irwin, Chicago, IL
    Nilsson GE (1999) Brain and body oxygen requirements of Gnathonemus petersii, a fish with an exceptionally large brain. J Exp Biol 199:603-607
    Nyagu AI, Loganovsky KN (1998) Neuro-psychiatric Effects of Ionizing Radiation. Chernobylinterinform, Kiev, Ukraine
    Portmann A (1947) Etudes sur la cérébralisation chez les oiseaux. Alauda 15:1-15
    Purvis A, Rambaut A (1995) Comparative analysis by independent contrasts (CAIC): an Apple-Macintosh application for analyzing comparative data. Comput Appl Biosci 11:247-251
    Ricklefs RE (2004) The cognitive face of avian life histories. Wilson Bull 116:119-133
    Rosenthal R (1994) Parametric measures of effect size. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV (eds) The Handbook of Research Synthesis. Russell Sage Foundation, New York, NY, pp 231-244
    Rutkowski R, Rejt L, Gryczynska-Siematkowska A, Jagolkowska P (2005) Urbanization gradient and genetic variability of birds: Example of kestrels in Warsaw. Berkut 14:130-136
    SAS Institute Inc (2012) JMP version 10. SAS Institute Inc, Cary
    Sewalk CJ, Brewer GL, Hoffman DJ (2001) Effects of diquat, an aquatic herbicide, on the development of mallard embryos. J Toxicol Environ Health A 62:33-45
    Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry. Freeman, New York, NY
    Sol D, Lefebvre L (2000) Behavioural flexibility predicts invasion success in birds introduced to New Zealand. Oikos 90:599-605
    Sol D, Timmermans S, Lefebvre L (2002) Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds. Anim Behav 63:495-502
    Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebvre L (2005) Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:5460-5465
    Stephan B (1999) Die Amsel. Neue Brehm-Bücherei. Wittenberg-Lutherstadt, Germany
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(10)

    1. Longwu Wang, Wei Liang. Random egg laying in host nests, rather than egg-matching, explains patterns of cuckoo parasitism: a comment on Zhang et al . (2023). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2023, 290(2006) DOI:10.1098/rspb.2023.1018
    2. Jinggang Zhang, Peter Santema, Zixuan Lin, et al. Experimental evidence that cuckoos choose host nests following an egg matching strategy. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2023, 290(1993) DOI:10.1098/rspb.2022.2094
    3. Manuel Azcárate-García, Silvia Díaz-Lora, Gustavo Tomás, et al. Spotless starlings prefer spotless eggs: conspecific brood parasites cue on eggshell spottiness to avoid ectoparasites. Animal Behaviour, 2020, 166: 33. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.05.017
    4. Wei Liang, Canchao Yang, Fugo Takasu. How can distinct egg polymorphism be maintained in the rufescent prinia (Prinia rufescens )-plaintive cuckoo (Cacomantis merulinus ) interaction-a modeling approach. Ecology and Evolution, 2017, 7(15): 5613. DOI:10.1002/ece3.3090
    5. Canchao Yang, Longwu Wang, Wei Liang, et al. How cuckoos find and choose host nests for parasitism. Behavioral Ecology, 2017, 28(3): 859. DOI:10.1093/beheco/arx049
    6. Federico Morelli, Anders Pape Møller, Emma Nelson, et al. Cuckoo as indicator of high functional diversity of bird communities: A new paradigm for biodiversity surrogacy. Ecological Indicators, 2017, 72: 565. DOI:10.1016/j.ecolind.2016.08.059
    7. Donglai Li, Yanan Ruan, Ying Wang, et al. Egg-spot matching in common cuckoo parasitism of the oriental reed warbler: effects of host nest availability and egg rejection. Avian Research, 2016, 7(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-016-0057-y
    8. Canchao Yang, Qiuli Huang, Longwu Wang, et al. Plaintive cuckoos do not select tailorbird hosts that match the phenotypes of their own eggs. Behavioral Ecology, 2016, 27(3): 835. DOI:10.1093/beheco/arv226
    9. Canchao Yang, Longwu Wang, Wei Liang, et al. Do common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) possess an optimal laying behaviour to match their own egg phenotype to that of their Oriental reed warbler (Acrocephalus orientalis) hosts?. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2016, 117(3): 422. DOI:10.1111/bij.12690
    10. Mominul Islam Nahid, Frode Fossøy, Sajeda Begum, et al. First record of Common Tailorbird (Orthotomus sutorius) parasitism by Plaintive Cuckoo (Cacomantis merulinus) in Bangladesh. Avian Research, 2016, 7(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-016-0049-y

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(3)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (223) PDF downloads (19) Cited by(10)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return