Yan Yang, Huan Wang, Zihui Zhang. 2015: Muscle architecture of the forelimb of the Golden Pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus) (Aves: Phasianidae) and its implications for functional capacity in flight. Avian Research, 6(1): 3. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0013-2
Citation: Yan Yang, Huan Wang, Zihui Zhang. 2015: Muscle architecture of the forelimb of the Golden Pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus) (Aves: Phasianidae) and its implications for functional capacity in flight. Avian Research, 6(1): 3. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0013-2

Muscle architecture of the forelimb of the Golden Pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus) (Aves: Phasianidae) and its implications for functional capacity in flight

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Zihui Zhang, zihuizhang@cnu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 13 Aug 2014
  • Accepted Date: 21 Jan 2015
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 28 Feb 2015
  • Background 

    Flight is the central avian adaptation in evolution. Wing muscles form an important anatomical basis for avian flight, affecting wing performance and determine modes of flight. However, the roles of distal muscles in adjusting the wing, as well as their functional specializations, remain largely unknown. The importance of muscle fiber architecture has long been recognized. In this study, we provide quantitative anatomical data on the muscle architecture of the forelimb of the Golden Pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus), with an emphasis on brachial, antebrachial and manual segments.

    Methods 

    The forelimbs of five Golden Pheasants were dissected and detailed measurements of all muscles were made, including muscle mass, muscle belly length, fascicle length. From these values, muscle volume, physiological cross-sectional area (PCSA) and maximum isometric force were derived.

    Results 

    General trends such as the distribution of muscle mass, fascicle length and the ratio of tendon length/belly length are revealed. Comparing PCSAs between antebrachial depressors and elevators and between intrinsics of the alular digit and major digit yielded significant differences (p < 0.05). Pronounced development of the antebrachial depressors suggests that ventral rotation of the distal half of the wing is a pivotal factor in shape change and orientation modulation. Large PCSAs in tandem with the force generation capability of the major digit intrinsics may help stabilize the digits while enhancing support of the primary feathers. The architectural properties of the alular digit confirm that alular adjustment is essential to rapid adduction and abduction.

    Conclusions 

    These observations illustrate the underlying structural basis for the functional capacities of the distal forelimb muscles and may provide additional information useful in further biomechanical and in vivo investigations.

  • Increasing urbanization worldwide has generated both greater awareness of the need to conserve urban biodiversity and an increase in the research required to inform that conservation process (Dunn et al. 2006; DeStefano and Degraaf 2008). Urbanization dramatically changes the natural environment; some species thrive under the altered circumstances ('urban exploiters' and 'adapters'), but others ('urban avoiders') are unable to adjust to the altered environment (Blair 2001). We need to understand what determines this disparity in order to successfully conserve urban biodiversity and ensure the survival of species sensitive to urbanization. One key to successful urban living by birds is an ability to tolerate high levels of disturbance by mostly harmless people (Price 2008). Urban living would not be viable, because critical activities would be curtailed, if birds: (a) spent a large proportion of the day monitoring their environment for stimuli posing little real threat to their survival, or (b) fled long distances rapidly in an energetically-expensive manner when approaching humans were still a long distance away (Frid and Dill 2002).

    Theoretically, the tolerance of human proximity necessary to facilitate urban living could eventuate in one of several ways: (a) cities might only be colonized by bird species whose members are all relatively bold as a result of natural selection for boldness in the exurban environment, (b) urban members of a colonizing species might rapidly become bolder than ex-urban con-specifics through genetic selection (Møller 2008), (c) cities might only be settled by bolder subsets of a species' members through a sorting process (Atwell et al. 2012), or (d) birds that colonize cities might adjust to high human population densities through behavioural plasticity that allows them to rapidly learn that urban humans mostly pose little threat and do not warrant extensive surveillance or early, rapid fleeing responses (Lima and Bednekoff 1999; Weston et al. 2012).

    Species that have genetically adapted to cities for a large number of generations should have shorter Flight Initiation Distances (FID) and smaller relative time allocations to anti-predator vigilance than recent urban colonists. By genetic adaptation we mean adjustment to inhabiting the urban environment that comes about through natural selection (i.e. beneficial genetic change). Møller (2008) demonstrated such a trend for FID in some Western European bird species, but few other studies have addressed this issue, particularly in Australia where cities tend to have a shorter history than many European conurbations. Even where tolerance of disturbing stimulation by humans in cities is primarily achieved through learning, if intra-specific observational, rather than experiental, learning is involved, we might still expect that a very recent urban colonist species would be less tolerant of human proximity than more established species. This would be so because the abundance of models from which to learn tolerance would presumably be relatively low during the colonization process. In birds, FID is a useful, easily-obtained metric of boldness and tolerance of disturbance by humans, with both theoretical and management significance (Weston et al. 2012; Møller et al. 2014; Vines and Lill 2015). Vigilance effort has been used less often in this context, but has potential as a tolerance metric (Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder 2003).

    The Rock Dove (Columba livia) is believed to have been introduced to Australia around the time of British settlement in 1788, but the first recorded releases in the state of Victoria were in the 1870s (Long 1981). It is an 'urban exploiter' that lives commensally with humans in cities in Australia and worldwide (Robbins 1995; Higgins and Davies 1996). Human food waste spillage and food deliberately provided by humans ('volunteer' food) comprise a significant proportion of its urban diet (Mulhall and Lill 2011). The Spotted Dove (Streptopelia chinensis), another urban exploiter, was possibly introduced to Melbourne in the 1860s, but definitely released in the city from 1870-1874 (Long 1981). It depends less on human food waste than the Rock Dove, but also consumes livestock food (Frith et al. 1976; Mulhall and Lill 2011). In contrast, the endemic Crested Pigeon (Ochyphaps lophotes) is an 'urban adapter' that is less commensal than the other two dove species and consumes only a small volume of anthropogenic food (Frith et al. 1974). It has settled in most major east coast temperate zone cities, but only colonized Melbourne's eastern suburbs in the last 15-20 years (A. Lill, personal observation).

    We predicted that in Melbourne the Crested Pigeon would be less adjusted to human proximity than the Rock Dove and Spotted Dove. Not only have the latter two species been established in urban Melbourne far longer than the Crested Pigeon, but the original founder individuals of at least the Rock Dove came from overseas populations that already had a substantial history of close contact with humans. We tested this prediction by contemporaneously: (a) comparing the FID and retreat behaviour of members of the three species to an approaching observer, and (b) measuring the mean proportion of daylight hours spent by the three species in vigilance (i.e. surveillance for threatening stimuli). We reasoned that the predicted lesser tolerance of disturbance by humans of Crested Pigeons would be manifested in fleeing from approaching people earlier than the other species and in spending a greater proportion of terrestrial foraging time in surveillance for approaching humans.

    The study was conducted in the austral autumn and winter (April-August, 2011) at 150 sites spread widely throughout the eastern suburbs of Melbourne, Australia (37.840S, 144.980E). The city has a human population of over 4 million and extends over 9, 900 km2. The study sites, mainly in streets, public parks, recreation reserves and on sports fields, were at least 1 km apart to preclude repeated sampling of unmarked individuals.

    Observations were made on all days of the week between 07:00-11:00 and 13:00-17:00 AEST. At each haphazardly chosen site, two observations per species on two different focal individuals were made for each behavioural variable, whenever possible. Only birds that were on the ground and active were recorded. In conjunction with the behavioural records, we also recorded categorically (0, 1-5 or > 5) several co-variates within 10 m of the focal bird that could potentially influence its behaviour: (1) refuges from predators (e.g. buildings or vegetation cover), (2) potentially threatening stimuli (e.g. predators or moving vehicles), (3) humans and (4) con-specifics and hetero-specific pigeons. Size of the flock of which the focal bird was a member was also noted. All these co-variates have been demonstrated to influence FID in various bird species (Blumstein 2006; Weston et al. 2012). Based on Australian Bureau of Statistics (2011) data, sites were roughly categorized as having an estimated low (≤500 people/km2) or high (> 2, 500 people/km2) human population density.

    The same observer conducted all FID measurements. She always approached the focal bird in a straight line at a steady pace (0.5-1 m per sec) and measured the distance (±1 m) between herself and the bird at which her approach stimulated it to retreat. The Starting Distance (distance from observer's starting point to focal bird's initial location) was also recorded. These distances were measured with a Bushnell Yardage Pro Sport 450™ laser rangefinder. The focal birds' mode of retreat (walking, running or flying) and the (unequal) categorical distances that they retreated (≤5, 5.1-10 and > 10 m) were also recorded. The second recording from a different flock-member was made at least 1 min after the first one to avoid 'carry-over' effects.

    Time investment by foraging doves in vigilance (head-up, visual scanning) was measured for focal individuals over 1-5 min periods using instantaneous sampling (Martin and Bateson 2007) with a 5-sec fixed interval. Co-variates were recorded as outlined above. A second recording from a different member of the same flock was made at least 30-sec after the first one. Although this could potentially reduce independence of the data, there are precedents for using this procedure for constructing time-activity budgets in flocking species (e.g. Magrath and Lill 1983). Vigilance records were obtained from a distance and at a time when the observer's presence or activity were unlikely to be influencing the focal bird's behaviour (McGiffin et al. 2013).

    Statistical analysis was conducted with R version 2.10.1. Data were inspected for conformity with the assumptions of the various significance tests used. To increase statistical power of some analyses, some co-variates were excluded and some categorical variables were pooled where it was evident that their inclusion did not contribute to the explanatory power of the analysis. Inclusion of second records from a flock did not affect the outcome of analyses, so we included all vigilance allocations and FIDs recorded. As starting distance had a strong influence on FID (see Blumstein 2003), we used adjusted FID (FIDadj), the residuals from an ordinary least squares regression of FID on starting distance, in analyses of tolerance of the doves of an approaching observer. Chi square contingency tests were used to examine species differences in mode and distance of retreat from an approaching observer.

    Due to the FID and vigilance data's non-linearity and the complexity of the behavioural interactions, we used non-linear Conditional Inference Tree and Random Forests models to characterise which variables and co-variates explained most of the variation in FIDadj and the time allocation to vigilance. Conditional Inference Tree models use non-biased splitting and are robust to co-linearity of the variables (Zuur et al. 2007). They progressively split the data into sub-groups with the smallest within-group variation and the largest between-group variation in the tree. Random Forests models are collections of categorical and regression tree analyses (Liaw and Wiener 2002) which can indicate the relative explanatory power of various predictor variables to explain variation in a response variable (Breiman 2001).

    Mean actual FIDs (m) of the three species were: Rock Dove 5.3±0.4, Spotted Dove 14±0.7 and Crested Pigeon 13.4±0.6. Conditional Inference Tree analysis (Figure 1) indicated that FIDadj was significantly shorter in the Rock Dove than in the other two species, which did not differ in approachability. In Rock Doves, FIDadj was also significantly shorter where estimated human population density was higher. Random Forests analysis indicated that for the three species considered collectively, species identity had the strongest proportional influence on FIDadj (76%), with estimated human population density (14%), habitat type and refuge type (each~7%) exerting much smaller influences. No other measured co-variate had any effect on FIDadj.

    Figure  1.  Conditional Inference Tree model for adjusted Flight Initiation Distance in three urban dove species.
    CP=Crested Pigeon, SD=Spotted Dove and RD=Rock Dove. Modelling conducted on arcsine square-root transformed data. Summary statistics for Nodes 2, 4 and 5 on a scale from -15 to +15. Q1 and Q3 are first and third quartiles, respectively. Minimum and maximum values do not include outliers.

    The three species varied in their mode of retreat from the approaching observer (χ2 (4)=102.5, P < 0.001) (Figure 2). Inspection of the standardized residuals (Table 1) indicates that Rock Doves walked away more and flew away less than expected, Crested Pigeons ran away more and flew away less than expected, and Spotted Doves flew away more but walked and ran away less than expected. The species also varied in the distance that they retreated from the observer (χ2 (4)=86.2, P < 0.001) (Figure 2). The standardized residuals (Table 1) indicate that Rock Doves fled≤5 m more and > 10 m less than expected, Spotted Doves fled > 10 m more and≤5 m less than expected and Crested Pigeons retreated 5.1-10 m more and > 10 m less than expected.

    Figure  2.  Retreat mode and distance of three urban dove species to an approaching observer.
    Black columns=Rock Dove, white columns=Spotted Dove and grey columns=Crested Pigeon. n=234.
    Table  1.  Standardised residuals for chi square analyses of retreat mode and distance of urban doves
    Species
    Retreat mode Crested Pigeon Rock Dove Spotted Dove
    Walk -0.8 +3.69 -3.21
    Run +4.2 -1.14 -2.91
    Fly -2.85 -2.82 +5.87
    Retreat distance (m)
    ≤ 5 +0.35 +3.49 -4.04
    5.1-10 +2.44 -1.63 -0.74
    > 10 -2.5 -3.05 +5.74
    Residuals > 2 in bold.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Rock Doves' ground-foraging occurred fairly equally in the quite open habitat of public parks (45.7% of observations) and the more visually obstructed habitat of streetscapes (43.3%), but much less on very open sports fields (11.0%). Spotted Doves' ground-foraging occurred mainly in streetscapes (78.5% of observations) and much less in parks (13.5%) and on sports fields (8.0%). Ground-foraging of Crested Pigeons took place mostly on sports fields (63.0% of observations), far less in more visually obstructed streetscapes (23.6%) and least in parks (13.4%).

    Conditional Inference Tree analysis indicated that the vigilance time allocation of ground-foraging Rock Doves (mean 6.0±0.9%, n=87) was significantly smaller than that of either Spotted Doves (15.4±1.5%, n=67) or Crested Pigeons (15.5±1.9%, n=87), whose allocations were similar (Figures 3 and 4). Vigilance allocation was also greater when flock size was≤5 (20.6±2% of time) than when it was > 5 individuals (10.7±1%) in Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons, but was unaffected by flock size in Rock Doves (Figure 4). Random Forests analysis indicated that for the three species considered collectively, species (40.0%) and flock size (25.0%) had the greatest proportional influences on vigilance allocation, with the presence of con-specifics also exerting about a 15% proportional influence. All other co-variates had a proportional influence≤5%.

    Figure  3.  Mean (± SE) proportional time spent in vigilance by three urban dove species.
    RD=Rock Dove, SD=Spotted Dove and CP=Crested Pigeon.
    Figure  4.  Conditional Inference Tree model for proportional time allocation to vigilance in three urban dove species.
    CP=Crested Pigeon, SD=Spotted Dove and RD=Rock Dove. Modelling conducted on arcsine square-root transformed data. Summary statistics for Nodes 3, 4 and 5 on a scale from 0 to 10. Q1 and Q3 are first and third quartiles, respectively. Minimum and maximum values do not include outliers.

    The Rock Dove had the shortest FIDadj to an approaching observer, the slowest dominant mode of retreat (walking) and the shortest principal retreat distance. However, contrary to our prediction, the Crested Pigeon had a similar FIDadj and vigilance time allocation to the Spotted Dove, and a slower dominant mode of retreat and shorter average retreat distance than the latter species.

    Among bird species, longer FIDs are variously associated with greater body size, older age at first reproduction, omnivory and carnivory, cooperative breeding and habitat 'openness' (Blumstein 2006). Among urban birds, species that have genetically adapted to cities for many generations have shorter FIDs than recent urban colonists (Møller 2008). However, for the species in the present study, none of these traits explained the observed disparities in FID. The largest species, the Rock Dove (34.5 cm mean total length), actually had a significantly smaller FIDadj than the other species, and the smallest species, the Spotted Dove (30.5 cm), had a similar FIDadj to the slightly larger Crested Pigeon (32.5 cm). All three related species are granivores, differing substantially in their diet only in the volume of human food waste consumed in the urban environment (Higgins and Davies 1996; Mulhall and Lill 2011). None of them has been recorded breeding cooperatively to any significant extent (Ford et al. 1988).

    Møller et al. (2014) showed that, particularly in Europe, farmland birds tended to have longer FIDs than those in other habitats. This may be related to both the declining bird population densities and the relative lack of cover (and hence birds' greater vulnerability to predation) in agricultural land. Rock Doves, which had the shortest mean FIDadj, foraged mainly and equally in fairly open public parks and more visually obstructed streetscapes. However, Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons, which had a similar mean FIDadj, contrasted in foraging habitat 'openness', the former mostly using streetscapes with many visual obstructions and the latter very open sports fields. The Rock Dove, which has probably been present in Melbourne longest (possibly for > 200 years), was the least wary of humans. However, whilst the Spotted Dove, which has been resident in the city probably for c. 150 years, was more wary of human proximity than the Rock Dove, it was also more wary than the very recent urban colonizer, the Crested Pigeon. Our comparison of three dove species in the urban environment substantially controlled for purely phylogenetic influences on tolerance of disturbance by humans. However, it must be conceded that it is probably less likely to reveal the particular influences on FID of the type listed above than a phylogenetically broader comparison.

    Natural selection for tolerance of human proximity may have occurred in urban Rock and Spotted Doves, but it cannot plausibly explain the fact that Crested Pigeons were more tolerant than Spotted Doves. It is possible that the observed pattern of tolerance occurred because Crested Pigeons as a species, or the subset of urban founder Crested Pigeons, were bolder than Spotted Doves even after generations of selection for tolerance in the latter species. However, it seems more likely that behavioural flexibility was involved in the greater tolerance exhibited by Crested Pigeons. Such greater flexibility could theoretically be related to relative brain size, which is significantly positively correlated with urban colonizing ability in some birds (Malakov et al. 2011), but was not compared among the present study species. However, Guay et al. (2013) have shown that at least in shorebirds brain volume is not correlated with FID. FID being shorter where estimated human population density was higher in the present investigation could be attributable to either habituation or differential settlement by individuals varying inherently in tolerance of disturbance.

    Anti-predator vigilance is another variable reflecting tolerance of human and predator proximity (Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder 2003). Greater investment in vigilance can increase the distance at which approaching predators and people are detected and/or the probability of detecting them. This enhanced detection ability gives the bird more flexibility to adaptively increase or decrease FID, depending on other relevant variables (e.g. perceived predation risk, Lima and Dill 1990; body mass, Creswell 1995). Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder (2003) showed that Spot-winged Pigeons (Columba maculosa) had a greater proportional time allocation to vigilance and a longer mean FID than co-habiting, smaller Eared Doves (Zenaida auriculata). Presumably for Spot-winged Doves the optimal adaptive response if predators are detected at a great distance is, for whatever reason, to flee early. In our investigation, Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons seemed to have this greater flexibility to adaptively adjust their FID. They had a significantly greater proportional vigilance allocation than Rock Doves and hence probably a superior ability to detect approaching predators and people, and they retreated from an approaching human significantly earlier than Rock Doves. As Rock Doves in Melbourne are directly fed by humans much more than the other two species, they may have down-regulated anti-predator surveillance as the risk posed by people is perceived as being smaller (Lima and Bednekoff 1999).

    The similar vigilance budgets of Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons are intriguing, because the former forages in a more visually obstructed habitat than the latter and consequently might be expected to spend more time being vigilant (Lazarus and Symonds 1992). However, Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder (2003) found that proportional visual scanning time actually decreased as visually obstructive tree cover increased for urban Spot-winged Pigeons and Eared Doves, so the greater level of visual obstruction in streetscapes might be contributing to the similarity in vigilance budgets of Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons. Conceivably, at high levels of visual obstruction vigilance may become relatively ineffective and be down-regulated. The observation that vigilance effort of Spotted Doves and Crested Pigeons was less in larger flocks accords with many, but certainly not all, findings for other bird species (Beauchamp 2008), but it is intriguing that the highly gregarious Rock Doves did not exhibit the same relationship. Currently we know too little about natural predation on these three urban doves to factor it into the vigilance formula.

    Whilst it is valid and informative to compare bird species' surveillance for predators by measuring relative vigilance time allocations, two qualifications are pertinent. First, some species' visual fields probably permit surveillance for predators during head-down foraging (Fernández-Juricic et al. 2008), although this may be less of an issue when, as here, closely-related species (which probably have similar visual fields) are being compared. Second, even when species' proportional vigilance time allocations are similar, differences in scanning regime structure may nonetheless result in disparate predator detection efficacies (Fernández-Juricic 2012). These aspects of vigilance require examination in the doves investigated here.

    The Rock Dove, probably the earliest of the study species to have colonised Melbourne, was the most tolerant of human disturbance, having the shortest FID and smallest proportional vigilance effort. However, the Crested Pigeon, the most recent of the three species to have colonised the city, was as, or more, tolerant of disturbance by humans as the second-longest resident, the Spotted Dove. We conclude that:

    (1) genetic selection cannot entirely explain the pattern of tolerance of disturbance by humans observed among the study species and behavioural flexibility appears likely to be involved

    (2) more generally, length of residency in a city is not an infallible guide to a species' relative level of tolerance of disturbance by humans

    (3) logically, we might predict that FID and vigilance effort would be positively correlated in bird species communities and Fernández-Juricic and Schroeder (2003) provide some supporting evidence for such a correlation. Our observations were also consistent with this prediction. Vigilance effort therefore has potential as a metric of tolerance of disturbance in urban birds. However, we still need much more information on the identity and abundance of natural predators in cities to fully understand the implications of interspecific variation in vigilance effort.

    Our study was essentially a 'snapshot' of the current tolerance of disturbance by humans of three dove species in the urban environment. Longitudinal studies of recent urban colonists and their exurban conspecifics would be very demanding, but would allow us to determine if and how tolerance of such disturbance alters through the various stages of urban colonization, from initial settlement to widespread establishment. This would help in defining the relative roles of genetic adaptation and behavioural flexibility in adjusting to disturbance by humans and identifying the specific influences on the acquisition of tolerance to such disturbance.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    JG collected all the field data and conducted much of the data analysis. AL was involved in project conception and management, some data analysis and wrote the manuscript. JB was involved in project conception and management. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    We thank Christopher P. Johnstone for statistical assistance. The study was approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the School of Biological Sciences, Monash University.

  • Alvarez JC, Meseguer J, Pérez A (2001) On the role of the alula in the steady flight of birds. Ardeola 48:161-173
    Askew GN, Marsh RL (2002) Muscle designed for maximum short-term power output: quail flight muscle. J Exp Biol 205:2153-2160
    Askew GN, Marsh RL, Ellington CP (2001) The mechanical power output of the flight muscles of blue-breasted quail (Coturnix chinensis) during take-off. J Exp Biol 204:3601-3619
    Berg AM, Biewener AA (2010) Wing and body kinematics of takeoff and landing flight in the pigeon (Columba livia). J Exp Biol 213:1651-1658
    Biewener AA (2011) Muscle function in avian flight: achieving power and control. Phil Trans R Soc B 366:1496-1506
    Brown NAT, Pandy MG, Kawcak CE, Mcilwraith CW (2003) Force- and moment-generating capacities of muscles in the distal forelimb of the horse. J Anat 203:101-113
    Channon AJ, Günther MM, Crompton RH, Vereecke EE (2009) Mechanical constraints on the functional morphology of the gibbon hind limb. J Anat 215:383-400
    Combes SA, Daniel TL (2001) Shape, flapping and flexion: wing and fin design for forward flight. J Exp Biol 204:2073-2085
    Corvidae EL, Bierregaard RO, Peters SE (2006) Comparison of wing morphology in three birds of prey: correlations with differences in flight behavior. J Morphol 267:612-622
    Dial KP (1992a) Avian forelimb muscles and nonsteady flight: can birds fly without using the muscles in their wings? Auk 109:874-885
    Dial KP (1992b) Activity patterns of the wing muscles of the pigeon (Columba livia) during different modes of flight. J Exp Zool 262:357-373
    Eng CM, Smallwood LH, Rainiero MP, Lahey M, Ward SR, Lieber RL (2008) Scaling of muscle architecture and fiber types in the rat hindlimb. J Exp Biol 211:2336-2345
    Fisher HI (1946) Adaptations and comparative anatomy of the locomotor apparatus of New World vultures. Am Midl Nat 35:545-727
    Gans C (1982) Fiber architecture and muscle function. Exercise Sport Sci Rev 10:160-207
    George JC, Berger AJ (1966) Avian myology. Academic, New York
    Gill FB (1995) Ornithology, 2nd edn. W. H. Freeman, New York
    Greenwalt CH (1962) Dimensional relationships for flying animals. Smithson Misc Coll 144:1-46
    Hildebrand M (1988) Form and function in vertebrate feeding and locomotion. Am Zool 28:727-738
    Kovacs CE, Meyers RA (2000) Anatomy and histochemistry of flight muscles in a wing-propelled diving bird, the Atlantic Puffin, Fratercula arctica. J Morphol 244:109-125
    Lei FM, Lu TC (2006) China endemic birds. Science Press, Beijing
    Lieber RL, Fridén J (2000) Functional and clinical significance of skeletal muscle architecture. Muscle Nerve 23:1647-1666
    Lieber RL, Ward SR (2011) Skeletal muscle design to meet functional demands. Phil Trans R Soc B 366:1466-1476
    McKitrick MC (1991) Forelimb myology of loons (Gaviiformes), with comments on the relationship of loons and tubenoses (Procellariiformes). Zool J Linn Soc 102:115-152
    Mendez J, Keys A (1960) Density and composition of mammalian muscle. Metabolism 9:184-188
    Meyers RA (1992) The morphological basis of folded-wing posture in the American Kestrel, Falco sparverius. Anat Rec 232:493-498
    Meyers RA, Stakebake EF (2005) Anatomy and histochemistry of spread-wing posture in birds. 3. immunohistochemistry of flight muscles and the "shoulder lock" in albatrosses. J Morphol 263:12-29
    Owre OT (1967) Adaptations for locomotion and feeding in the Anhinga and the Double-crested Cormorant. Ornithol Monographs 6:1-138
    Payne RC, Hutchinson JR, Robilliard JJ, Smith NC, Wilson AM (2005) Functional specialisation of pelvic limb anatomy in horses (Equus caballus). J Anat 206:557-574
    Payne RC, Crompton RH, Isler K, Savage R, Vereecke EE, Günther MM, Thorpe SKS, D'Août K (2006) Morphological analysis of the hindlimb in apes and humans. I. Muscle architecture. J Anat 208:709-724
    Poore SO, Ashcroft A, Sánchez-Haiman A, Goslow GE Jr (1997) The contractile properties of the M. supracoracoideus in the pigeon and starling: a case for long-axis rotation of the humerus. J Exp Biol 200:2987-3002
    Roberts TJ (2002) The integrated function of muscles and tendons during locomotion. Comp Biochem Phys A 133:1087-1099
    Rupert JE, Rose JA, Organ JM, Butcher MT (2014) Forelimb muscle architecture and myosin isoform composition in the groundhog (Marmota monax). J Exp Biol. doi:
    Sacks RD, Roy RR (1982) Architecture of the hindlimb muscles of cats: functional significance. J Morphol 173:185-195
    Smith NC, Wilson AM, Jespers KJ, Payne RC (2006) Muscle architecture and functional anatomy of the pelvic limb of the ostrich (Struthio camelus). J Anat 209:765-779
    Steudel K (1996) Limb morphology, bipedal gait, and the energetics of hominid locomotion. Am J Phys Anthropol 99:345-355
    Swaddle JP, Lockwood R (2003) Wingtip shape and flight performance in the European Starling Sturnus vulgaris. Ibis 145:457-464
    Tobalske BW, Dial KP (2000) Effects of body size on take-off flight performance in the Phasianidae (Aves). J Exp Biol 203:3319-3332
    Torrella JR, Fouces V, Palomeque J, Viscor G (1998) Comparative skeletal muscle fibre morphometry among wild birds with different locomotor behaviour. J Anat 192:211-222
    Ward SR, Eng CM, Smallwood LH, Lieber RL (2009) Are current measurements of lower extremity muscle architecture accurate? Clin Orthop Relat Res 467:1074-1082
    Welch KC, Altshuler DL (2009) Fiber type homogeneity of the flight musculature in small birds. Comp Biochem Phys B 152:324-331
    Wells JB (1965) Comparison of mechanical properties between slow and fast mammalian muscle. J Physiol 178:252-269
    Williams SB, Wilson AM, Payne RC (2007) Functional specialisation of the thoracic limb of the hare (Lepus europeus). J Anat 210:491-505
    Williams SB, Wilson AM, Rhodes L, Andrews J, Payne RC (2008) Functional anatomy and muscle moment arms of the pelvic limb of an elite sprinting athlete: the racing greyhound (Canis familiaris). J Anat 213:361-372
    Zhang ZH, Yang Y (2013) Forelimb myology of the golden pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus). Int J Morphol 31:1482-1490
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(12)

    1. Takeshi Honda, Hironori Ueda. Why Mammals do Not Damage Entire Farmlands Like Insect Pests Do? A Review from a Behavioral Perspective. Mammal Study, 2023, 48(2) DOI:10.3106/ms2022-0054
    2. Luqin Yin, Cheng Wang, Wenjing Han, et al. Birds’ Flight Initiation Distance in Residential Areas of Beijing Are Lower than in Pristine Environments: Implications for the Conservation of Urban Bird Diversity. Sustainability, 2023, 15(6): 4994. DOI:10.3390/su15064994
    3. Melissa Ardila-Villamizar, Gustavo Alarcón-Nieto, Adriana A. Maldonado-Chaparro. Fear in urban landscapes: conspecific flock size drives escape decisions in tropical birds. Royal Society Open Science, 2022, 9(11) DOI:10.1098/rsos.221344
    4. Swaroop Patankar, Ravi Jambhekar, Kulbhushansingh Ramesh Suryawanshi, et al. Which Traits Influence Bird Survival in the City? A Review. Land, 2021, 10(2): 92. DOI:10.3390/land10020092
    5. Takeshi Honda. Geographical personality gradient in herbivorous animals: Implications for selective culling to reduce crop damage. Ecological Research, 2021, 36(1): 145. DOI:10.1111/1440-1703.12186
    6. Takeshi Honda, Yohichi Kubota, Yutaka Ishizawa. Ungulates-exclusion grates as an adjoining facility to crop damage prevention fences. European Journal of Wildlife Research, 2020, 66(1) DOI:10.1007/s10344-020-1362-7
    7. Daniel T. Blumstein. What chasing birds can teach us about predation risk effects: past insights and future directions. Journal of Ornithology, 2019, 160(2): 587. DOI:10.1007/s10336-019-01634-1
    8. Jenna L. Van Donselaar, Jenna L. Atma, Zachary A. Kruyf, et al. Urbanization alters fear behavior in black-capped chickadees. Urban Ecosystems, 2018, 21(6): 1043. DOI:10.1007/s11252-018-0783-5
    9. Javier delBarco-Trillo. Shyer and larger bird species show more reduced fear of humans when living in urban environments. Biology Letters, 2018, 14(4): 20170730. DOI:10.1098/rsbl.2017.0730
    10. T. Honda, H. Iijima, J. Tsuboi, et al. A review of urban wildlife management from the animal personality perspective: The case of urban deer. Science of The Total Environment, 2018, 644: 576. DOI:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.06.335
    11. Matthew R. E. Symonds, Michael A. Weston, Wouter F. D. van Dongen, et al. Time Since Urbanization but Not Encephalisation Is Associated with Increased Tolerance of Human Proximity in Birds. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2016, 4 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2016.00117
    12. Bao-Sen Shieh, Shih-Hsiung Liang, Yuh-Wen Chiu, et al. Interspecific comparison of traffic noise effects on dove coo transmission in urban environments. Scientific Reports, 2016, 6(1) DOI:10.1038/srep32519

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(2)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (431) PDF downloads (9) Cited by(12)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return