Lifang Gao, Wen Zhang, Wenjing Zhu, Yichen Wu, Ran Xu, Ningning Sun, Yujie Wang, Biyun Jia, Bo Du. 2025: Inability of recognizing offspring underlies parental errors in the selection of offspring. Avian Research, 16(1): 100218. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2024.100218
Citation: Lifang Gao, Wen Zhang, Wenjing Zhu, Yichen Wu, Ran Xu, Ningning Sun, Yujie Wang, Biyun Jia, Bo Du. 2025: Inability of recognizing offspring underlies parental errors in the selection of offspring. Avian Research, 16(1): 100218. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2024.100218

Inability of recognizing offspring underlies parental errors in the selection of offspring

Funds: 

Financial support was provided by the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China 32071491

Financial support was provided by the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China 31772465

Financial support was provided by the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China 31672299

Financial support was provided by the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China 31572271

Financial support was provided by the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China 32260128

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    E-mail address: dubo@lzu.edu.cn (B. Du)

  • Received Date: 07 Jan 2024
  • Rev Recd Date: 26 Sep 2024
  • Accepted Date: 25 Nov 2024
  • Publish Date: 29 Nov 2024
  • Inclusive fitness theory posits that altruistic behaviors, which are directed more likely towards relatives, should be favored by natural selection. However, the prevalence of alternative parenting behaviors in offspring selection, including rejecting their own offspring and accepting the offspring of others, remains poorly understood within the context of parental care evolution. In order to investigate the factors that prompt the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors, we designed a series of experiments in the Azure-winged Magpie (Cyanopica cyanus). By manipulating the nest spatial position or offspring age/number and parent-offspring familiarity, we addressed how parents provided parental care for the manipulated offspring. In the nest resettlement experiment, the probability of parents rejecting their own offspring significantly increased with nest-moving distances while decreased with offspring ages. In the cross-fostering experiments, the probability of parents provisioning unrelated young significantly decreased with the age difference between cross-fostered chicks. In the nest duplication experiments, where parents were given a choice between familiar offspring and unfamiliar unrelated chicks or between unfamiliar offspring and familiar unrelated chicks, the probability of both alternative parenting behaviors was significantly influenced by the time when parental association with their offspring was deprived. We conclude that as offspring phenotypic traits become individualized and fixed at a special developmental stage, parents gradually acquire the capacity for offspring recognition by associating with them. Any factors that disrupt parent–offspring association or introduce unrelated young into the nest prior to this critical timeline can result in the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors.

  • The theory of inclusive fitness highlights the significance of kinship as a fundamental explanation for the occurrence of social behaviors (Hamilton, 1964). Regarding the evolution of parental care, parenting behaviors directed towards their own offspring have undergone positive selection and should have been favored by natural selection (Royle et al., 2012). However, alternative parenting behaviors, such as rejecting their own offspring and accepting unrelated offspring, are widely observed across various animal species and life-history stages (Trivers, 1974). For example, in cases of extra-pair copulations (EPCs), cuckolded males may abandon the mixed brood, including their own offspring (Møller and Birkhead, 1993; Griffith et al., 2022; Perlut et al., 2012). In cases of brood parasitism or semi-parasitism, host parents often provision the parasitic young in their nests (Sorenson and Payne, 2002; Bouwman et al., 2005; Du et al., 2015). Obviously, both alternative parenting behaviors will impose negative effects on parental fitness (Hamilton, 1964). To demonstrate the prevalence of alternative parenting behaviors across animal species, it need to identify the potential effect of offspring recognition capacity on parental care behaviors, as well as potential factors facilitating the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors.

    Parental capacity for offspring recognition can influence their tradeoffs between rejecting and accepting a young. For instance, in the cases of EPCs, parental care strategies of cuckolded males depend largely on whether they can distinguish their own offspring from the extra-pair young. If they are able to recognize their own offspring, the optimal parenting behavior will be selectively providing care for their own offspring. They provision extra-pair young only when the presence of unrelated young is beneficial to their offspring, such as maintaining thermal stability in the nest (Smith et al., 2018). If cuckolded males are unable to recognize their own offspring, provisioning unrelated young occurs only when they have sired the majority of mixed paternity and the benefits of provisioning unrelated young outweigh the cost of abandoning the current brood (García-Vigón et al., 2009; Du et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2020). Otherwise, cuckolded males will probably reduce paternal care for the mixed brood (Møller and Birkhead, 1993; Perlut et al., 2012). Therefore, it is imperative to ascertain the parental capacity for offspring recognition as a prerequisite for demonstrating the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors.

    Parental capacity for offspring recognition is closely associated with whether offspring can give out an individualized, fixed signal (Dowling et al., 2016). It has been reported in vertebrates that various phenotypic traits have the potential as the recognition cue, including acoustic features (Malmkvist, 2019), facial plumage (Johnson and Freedberg, 2014), olfactory odor (Mateo, 2017), and chemical fingerprints (Pitcher et al., 2015). These traits change remarkably with offspring growth (Mak and Weiss, 2010; Johnson and Freedberg, 2014). The ontogeny of offspring phenotypic traits indicates that parents depend heavily on an early association with their offspring so that they can learn the developmental status of offspring traits (Luther, 2008). In this scenario, any factors that disrupt parent–offspring association or introduce unrelated young into the nest before offspring phenotypic traits fully develop can impact parental capacity for offspring recognition. It is crucial to consider this influence when demonstrating the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors.

    In order to investigate the factors prompting the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors, we conducted a series of experiments by manipulating the nest and offspring signals in a Tibetan population of the Azure-winged Magpie (Cyanopica cyanus). EPCs occur frequently in the Tibetan Azure-winged Magpies and cuckolded males do not reduce but instead continuously provide parental care for the mixed brood (Gao et al., 2020a). The indiscrimination of Azure-winged Magpies in providing parental care for their own offspring and unrelated young provides us a good system to address the role of parental capacity of offspring recognition in determining their parental care strategies.

    In this study, three types of experiments were designed to examine parental reactions to the manipulated offspring. In the nest resettlement experiment, we manipulated only the nest spatial position to test whether nest-moving distance and offspring age influenced the occurrence of parental rejecting their own offspring. In the cross-fostering experiment, we exchanged the entire brood between two nests to test whether the changed number and age of offspring influenced the occurrence of parental accepting unrelated young. In the nest duplication experiment, we provided parents a choice between familiar offspring and unfamiliar unrelated chicks, as well as that between unfamiliar offspring and familiar unrelated chicks, which enabled us to verify the effects of time depriving parent–offspring association on the occurrence of both alternative parenting behaviors.

    This study was conducted in Luqu County, Gansu Province of China (34.6°N, 102.5° E, elevation 3400 m a.s.l.), from 2012 to 2021. The typical landscape of this region is alpine meadow, with clusters of shrubs, such as Berberis hemsleyana, Hippophae rhamnoides and Salix caprea, dispersed along the bank of the Tao River. Azure-winged Magpies constructed their nests mainly in the shrubs of H. rhamnoides and S. caprea. Constrained by their preference for shrub-nesting, the Tibetan population comprised of a string of colonies. In each colony, breeders nested in a highly-clumped pattern (20–180 nests/ha; Ren et al., 2016). As summer residents of the Tibetan Plateau, Azure-winged Magpies overwinter in the low-altitude regions, and migrate to the high-altitude habitat to breed. Experienced breeders usually returned to their previous colony and selected nest-sites near their territory from the previous year.

    Life-history data collection was carried out every year according to Ren et al. (2016). Eggs and nestlings were marked according to their laying/hatching orders, and measured every 1–3 days until they fledged or disappeared because of nest predation or starvation. Parenting behaviors of provisioning nestlings in the nest were recorded using a digital camcorder (ZX1; Eastman Kodak Company), which as fixed 1 m above the nest ever since adults were captured and banded to allow individual identification (Da et al., 2018). Video recordings for each nest lasted for at least three discontinuous days. Data on parental behaviors were extracted from the videos to determine whether parents in a focus nest provided parental care and which chicks were provisioned (Zhu et al., 2023).

    Three experiments were designed, namely nest resettlement, cross-fostering and nest duplication. In the nest resettlement experiment, a nest was manipulated by vertically moving it 0.5–6 m downwards to a new position. The branch supporting the nest was carefully severed and lowered to an appropriate height before being reattached to the remaining branch using sticks and metal wire upon the completion of the experiment (Fig. 1). To facilitate ease of manipulation, only nests situated on vertical stems of shrubs at heights ranging from 6 to 8 m above ground level were selected. Depending on the precise location of each nest along the stem, we ensured that its new position maintained a minimum distance of 2 m from ground level. The nest-moving and resettling procedure was completed in 5 min on sunny days to reduce the potential effect of low temperature on the offspring. Then, parental behaviors were monitored in subsequent half an hour. As Azure-winged Magpies always returned to their nests in less than 10 min during we checked the nest content, half an hour is enough for us to determine whether parents rejected their own offspring or not. Behavioral observations indicated that parents in all the experiments had returned to the manipulated nest: if parents did not resume their interrupted parenting behaviors, they were considered to reject their own offspring; if parents continued to incubate eggs or provision chicks, they were considered to have the capacity for offspring recognition. To exclude the possibility that parents rejected their own offspring because of the interference caused by the nest-moving protocol, the cut branch was reinstated to its original position after the experiment was ended, parental responses in the reinstated nest were recorded and used as a control. Behavioral observations indicated that parents in all the experiments had returned to the reinstated nest and resumed their parenting behaviors after the experiment, implying that the nest-moving protocol was not the reason leading some parents to reject their own offspring. In this study, the experiment was repeated in 96 nests at the egg stage (Fig. 1A) and in 84 nests at the chick stage (Fig. 1B), respectively.

    Figure  1.  Design of the nest resettlement experiment at the egg (A) or chick stage (B). (C) the probability of rejecting their own offspring increased with the nest-moving distance, (D) the probability of rejecting their own offspring decreased with offspring age. Procedures: (1) cutting off the branch to move the nest downwards, (2) reinstating the cut branch after the experiment as a control.

    In the cross-fostering experiment, all offspring between two nests were cross-fostered in three forms, both at the egg stage, both at the chick stage, or one at the egg stage and the other at the chick stage (Fig. 2AC), without controlling for the number and age of offspring. This experiment facilitated us to calculate the probability of parents providing care for unrelated young because only foreign offspring were provided to adults. If parents returned to the manipulated nest and incubated foreign eggs or provisioned foreign chicks in subsequent half an hour, they were considered to accept unrelated young; if parents returned to the manipulated nest but did not incubate foreign eggs or provisioned foreign chicks, they were considered to have the capacity for offspring recognition. In this study, we cross-fostered a total of 23 pairs of nests both at the egg stage, 27 pairs of nests both at the chick stage, and 25 pairs of nests at the egg and chick stage.

    Figure  2.  Design of the three cross-fostering experiments. (A) two nests both at the egg stage, (B) two nests both at the chick stage, (C) two nests at the egg and chick stage, respectively, (D) the probability of provisioning unrelated young did not vary with the changed incubation days in the first experiment, (E) the probability of provisioning unrelated young decreased with the changed age of cross-fostered chicks in the second experiment, (F) the probability of provisioning unrelated young did not vary with the incubation days but decreased with the age of cross-fostered chicks in the third experiment. Black line indicates parents at incubation, and the red line indicates parents in offspring provisioning. Procedure: cross-fostering the offspring between two nests.

    In the nest duplication experiment, an artificial nest was first constructed using an abandoned nest cup and fixed it to a new position with 0.5 m distance from a focus nest (hereafter referred to duplicated and original nest). According to the results of the nest-resettlement experiment, where parents consistently provided parental care for their offspring in all instances when the nest was relocated by 0.5 m, we ensured that the distance of 0.5 m between the duplicated nest and the original nest site did not influence whether parents provisioned or refused to provision chicks in the duplicated nest. Thereafter, two kinds of procedures were carried out. The first provided parents a choice between familiar offspring and unfamiliar unrelated chicks. During the experiment, all chicks in the original nest were transferred to the duplicated nest, and then same-aged chicks from another nest were transferred to the original nest (Fig. 3A). In this study, we successfully carried out 96 replicates for the first nest duplication experiment that covered various nestling ages. The second provided parents a choice between unfamiliar offspring and familiar unrelated chicks. Originally, new hatchlings in the original nest were cross-fostered with the same-aged hatchlings in another nest so that all chicks were raised by their foster parents. After they were 11 days old, one or two familiar, unrelated chicks were transferred to the duplicated nest, and the same-numbered unfamiliar offspring were transferred to the original nest (Fig. 3B). Such a design could exclude the possibility that parents would inevitably reject some chicks if there were too many chicks begged simultaneously in one provisioning process. To minimize the effect of brood-mates' begging on parental selection, only fostered chicks were kept in the original and duplicated nests, and their brood-mates were temporarily raised in an artificial nest during the experiment. In this study, we successfully carried out 16 replicates for the second nest duplication experiment that used only 11-day-old chicks. Parental behaviors were monitored by the way described above in subsequent an hour of transferring the chicks. In the first nest duplication experiment, if parents only provisioned the chicks in the duplicated nest, they were considered to have the ability of recognizing their offspring; if parents provisioned the chicks in the original nest, they were considered to accept unrelated young, irrespective of whether they provisioned the chicks in the duplicated nest. In the second nest duplication experiment, if parents provisioned the unfamiliar offspring in the original nest, they were considered to have the capacity for offspring recognition; if parents provisioned the familiar unrelated chicks in the duplicated nest but not the unfamiliar offspring in the original nest, they were considered to make both errors. By simultaneously providing genetic offspring and unrelated chicks in the original and duplicated nest, this experiment facilitated us to determine whether parents have ability of recognizing their offspring.

    Figure  3.  Design of two nest duplication experiments. (A) parents selecting between familiar offspring and unfamiliar unrelated young, procedures: (1) adding the duplicated nest 0.5 m near the original nest, (2) transferring the familiar offspring to the duplicated nest, (3) transferring the unfamiliar unrelated chicks to the original nest. (B) parents selecting between unfamiliar offspring and familiar cross-fostered chicks, procedures: (1) cross-fostering new hatchlings between two nests, (2) adding the duplicated nest 0.5 m near the original nest, (3) transferring the familiar cross-fostered chicks to the duplicated nest, (4) transferring the unfamiliar offspring to the original nest. (C) the probability of provisioning unrelated young decreased with nestling age in the first nest duplication experiment. (D) the frequency of parents provisioning familiar cross-fostered chicks and unfamiliar offspring in the second nest duplication experiment.

    Generalized linear models (GLMs) were fitted to test factors that might influence whether parents returned and resumed their interrupted parenting behaviors in the manipulated nest (binomial distribution, with logit link function). In the nest resettlement experiment, predictor variables included the age of offspring (i.e., the incubation days for eggs, and the hatching days for chicks), nest-moving distance, and the breeding stage when the experiment was carried out (1 = at the egg stage, 0 = at the chick stage). In the cross-fostering experiment between two nests at the egg stage, predictor variables included the changed number of eggs and incubation days. In the cross-fostering experiment between two nests both at the chick stage, predictor variables included the age of parents' own offspring, and the age difference between cross-fostered chicks. In the cross-fostering experiment between two nests of eggs and chicks, predictor variables included the number of offspring, the changed number of offspring, and the age of offspring. In the first nest duplication experiment, predictor variables included the number and age of chicks. In the second nest duplication experiment, the frequency of parents provisioning familiar unrelated young and unfamiliar offspring were compared using chi-square test. During the fitting of GLMs, the predicted probability of parents rejecting their own offspring or providing parental care for unrelated young in the experiment was restored as a new variable and then regressed against the independent variable with significant effect on the dependent variable.

    All statistical analyses were carried out using R (Version 3.4.4). Descriptive results are presented as mean ± standard effort of the mean (SEM). The null hypothesis, positing an equal probability for parents to return and resume their interrupted parenting behaviors in a manipulated nest, will be rejected when the two-tailed P-value is less than 0.05.

    In the nest resettlement experiments (Fig. 1), 27.37% (26/95) of incubation parents and 58.33% (49/84) of provisioning parents returned to the manipulated nests and resumed their interrupted parenting behaviors. In the control, all parents returned to the reinstated nests and resumed their parenting behaviors. Whether parents resumed their interrupted parenting behaviors in a manipulated nest was significantly related to the nest-moving distance and offspring age, and it differed significantly between at the egg and chick stage (Table 1). The larger the distance of nest-moving (Fig. 1C) and the younger the offspring (Fig. 1D), the higher the probability that parents rejected their own offspring. The probability of parents rejecting their own offspring was higher at the egg stage than at the chick stage (Fig. 1C and D).

    Table  1.  Results of fitting generalized linear model to test factors influencing whether parents resumed their parenting behaviors to the manipulated nest in the nest resettlement experiment.
    Predictor variables β ± SE n z P
    Intercept 3.30 ± 1.75 179 1.88 0.06
    Offspring age (day) 0.34 ± 0.08 179 4.36 < 0.001
    Nest-moving distance (m) −2.44 ± 0.39 179 −6.22 < 0.001
    At the chick stagea 4.91 ± 0.82 84 6.01 < 0.001
    aNote: the value at the egg stage was set as the baseline (n = 95).
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In the cross-fostering experiments, when two nests at the egg stage were cross-fostered, 67.39% of females (31 of 46; χ2 = 5.57, P = 0.018) returned to incubate foreign eggs. Whether females resumed their incubation behaviors was unrelated to the changed number of eggs or incubation days (Table 2), suggesting a consistently high probability for parents to accept unrelated offspring at the egg stage (Fig. 2D). When two nests at the chick stage were cross-fostered, 83.33% of parents (45 of 54 nests, χ2 = 24.00, P < 0.001) returned to the nest and provisioned foreign chicks. Whether parents resumed their provisioning behaviors was unrelated to nestling age but significantly related to the changed age between cross-fostered chicks (Table 2). The probability of parents provisioning unrelated young decreased with the age difference between fostered chicks (Fig. 2E).

    Table  2.  Results of fitting generalized linear model to test factors influencing whether parents provided parental care for the cross-fostered eggs or chicks in the cross-fostering experiments.
    Stage Predictor variables β ± SE n z P
    Both nests at the egg stage Intercept −0.73 ± 0.31 46 −2.31 0.02
    Changed number of eggs 0.05 ± 0.23 46 0.22 0.83
    Changed incubation days 0.001 ± 0.05 46 0.02 0.98
    Both nests at the chick stage Intercept −9.87 ± 3.95 32 −2.50 0.01
    Age of the fostered nestlings 0.31 ± 0.19 32 1.64 0.10
    Changed age of the fostered nestlings 0.79 ± 0.38 32 2.06 0.04
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    When eggs in a nest were cross-fostered with chicks in another nest, incubation parents advanced their parenting behaviors from incubation to provisioning in 92.00% of nests (23 of 25 nests; χ2 = 17.64, P < 0.001), while provisioning parents reverted their parenting behaviors to incubation in 76.00% of nests (19 of 25 nests; χ2 = 6.76, P = 0.009). Whether incubation parents carried out the provisioning of foreign chicks was unrelated to incubation days of eggs, the number or changed number of offspring (Table 3). Whether provisioning parents carried out the incubation of foreign eggs was unrelated to the number or changed number of offspring, but significantly related to the age of their own chicks (Table 3). Parents had a higher possibility to advance than to revert their parenting behaviors in this cross-fostering experiment (Fig. 2F). The probability of provisioning parents accepting unrelated eggs decreased significantly with the age of their own chicks (Fig. 2F).

    Table  3.  Results of fitting generalized linear models to test factors influencing whether parents provided parental care for the cross-fostered offspring in the experiments of cross-fostering eggs and chicks.
    Predictor variables Parameters of the model
    At the egg stage (n = 25) At the chick stage (n = 25)
    β ± SE z P β ± SE z P
    Intercept 0.14 ± 4.78 0.03 0.98 −4.85 ± 4.77 −1.02 0.31
    Number of nestlings −0.43 ± 0.72 −0.59 0.55 0.17 ± 0.81 0.21 0.84
    Changed number of nestlings −1.10 ± 1.02 −1.08 0.28 −0.02 ± 0.74 −0.03 0.98
    Nestling age 0.28 ± 0.21 1.33 0.19 0.46 ± 0.21 2.22 0.03
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In the first nest duplication experiment, the familiar offspring were situated in the duplicated nest while the unfamiliar unrelated chicks were positioned in the original nest. The focus parents delivered food to the duplicated nest in 53 cases and to the original nest in 43 cases (χ2 = 1.04, df = 1, P = 0.307). Whether parents provisioned their own offspring was significantly related to the age but not number of their offspring (Table 4). The probability of parents provisioning unrelated young decreased significantly with the age of their offspring (Fig. 3C).

    Table  4.  Results of fitting generalized linear model to test factors influencing whether parents provisioned their own offspring in the first nest duplication experiment.
    Fixed effects β ± SE n z P
    Intercept −1.20 ± 1.16 96 −1.04 0.30
    Number of nestlings −0.05 ± 0.24 96 −0.22 0.82
    Age of parents' offspring 0.17 ± 0.08 96 2.08 0.04
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In the second nest duplication experiment, the familiar cross-fostered chicks were situated in the duplicated nest while the unfamiliar offspring in the original nest. The focus parents delivered food to the duplicated nest in fifteen cases and only once to the original nest (Fig. 3D; χ2 = 12.25, df = 1, P < 0.001). Obviously, this result differed significantly from that in the first nest duplication experiment.

    As offspring phenotypic traits would become individualized and fixed after a special developmental stage, parents could acquire the capacity for offspring recognition gradually by associating with them. The present study revealed that any factors that deprived parental association with their offspring or introduced unrelated young into the nest prior to this critical timeline would influence parental recognition of their offspring, hence leading to the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors.

    Individuals breeding in environments with food scarcity or higher risk of predation may exhibit an alternative parenting behavior during offspring provisioning (e.g., rejecting a partial of nestlings to ensure the survival of the remaining; Stoleson and Beissinger, 1997; Forbes et al., 2002; Zhu et al., 2023). These widespread phenomena indicate that rejecting their own offspring is actually a condition-dependent decision for parents, irrespective of whether they have the capacity for offspring recognition. For instance, parent birds are more likely to abandon current brood with the increasing risk of nest predation (Martin, 1995). In the present study, our nest-moving procedure in the nest resettlement experiment, i.e., the alteration of nest position (Fig. 1A and B), does look like a scene of nest predation. From the experimental results alone, there is a possibility that parental responses to risk can also result in the increased probability of rejecting their own offspring with nest-moving distances (Fig. 1C). However, as Azure-winged Magpies in our study area always experience high pressure of nest predation caused by predators or conspecific nest-raiders (Ren et al., 2016; Gao et al., 2022) and parents will not abandon the nest even if there is only one egg or chick left, it will be difficult for using parental responses to risk to explain why parents rejected their own offspring in a manipulated nest in the experiment. Alternatively, parental dependence on nest spatial position in offspring recognition is relatively a better explanation: along with the increase of nest-moving distance, the certainty that parents perceive a manipulated nest as their own will be lowered. Consequently, their probability of rejecting a manipulated nest increased (Fig. 1C).

    It has been widely observed in animals that parental capacity for offspring recognition exhibits a temporal variation with offspring growth. For instance, parents in Bank Swallow (Riparia riparia) (Beecher et al., 1981), mammals (Jovanovic et al., 2000) and even social insects (Müller and Eggert, 1990), have reduced their dependence on nest spatial position in recognizing their own offspring, they instead rely largely on offspring phenotypic traits. In the present study, parental probability of rejecting their own offspring deceased with nestling age (Fig. 1D) provided new evidence for such a pattern in the Azure-winged Magpie. As offspring phenotypic traits become individualized and fixed with their growth, parents can acquire the capacity for offspring recognition by learning the developmental status of offspring phenotypic traits.

    Parents accepting unrelated offspring may attribute to two reasons: one is that they are unable to recognize offspring, such as in cases of nest parasitism (Sorenson and Payne, 2002; Bouwman et al., 2005); the other is that parents are able to recognize offspring but recognizing and rejecting unrelated young is costly for them. For instances, when their own offspring and unrelated young with similar phenotypic traits are mixed together, the rejection of a suspected unrelated young increases the risk of rejecting their own offspring (Cherry and Bennett, 2001; Soler et al., 2003; Akkaynak and Stoddard, 2023). In our cross-fostering experiments conducted in Tibetan Azure-winged Magpies, we adopted two new ideas different from previous studies. Firstly, we cross-fostered the entire brood between two nests, which ensured that parents were exposed solely to unrelated chicks. Secondly, we established a gradient of age differences among cross-fostered offspring, even exchanging eggs with chicks, to ensure that parents could detect the change of offspring signals. If parents have the capacity for offspring recognition, they should reject the foreign eggs or chicks in the experiment. Our results showed that parents tended to incubate foreign eggs (Fig. 2D and E) and to provision foreign chicks at early chick stage (Fig. 2E and F) but did not exhibit the same patterns any more at later stage. These results further provided supports for an increased parental capacity for offspring recognition with the growth of offspring.

    Regarding the second explanation for why parents accept unrelated young, namely that recognizing and rejecting unrelated offspring incurs significant costs for parents, in terms of the survival of their own offspring, our cross-fostering experiments do not support this hypothesis. By cross-fostering the entire brood between two nests, we ensured that parents were exclusively exposed to unrelated chicks. If parents were capable of recognizing their own offspring, they would have abandoned the nest due to the complete absence of their own offspring in a manipulated nest.

    The cross-fostering procedure has been widely employed as a method for investigating the influence of parental effects on the development of offspring phenotypic traits and behaviors (Li and Zhang, 2010). Previous studies have indicated that the outcomes of cross-fostering experiments are closely associated with the initiation time of the experiment (Lefevre et al., 1998; Becciu et al., 2021), which can be attributed to parent−offspring association. Prior to establishing a strong parent−offspring association, parental effects on offspring development may exhibit greater strength, whereas after such an association is established, this effect may significantly diminish. In the Azure-winged Magpie, we designed cross-fostering experiments involving different-aged offspring, aiming to demonstrate how the time of depriving parent−offspring association affected parental recognition of foreign offspring. Results in both Fig. 2EF and Fig. 3C indicate that when parents were deprived association with their offspring at early chick stage, they did not acquire the capacity for offspring recognition but remained depending on the nest spatial position in locating their offspring. As a result, their probability of accepting unrelated young maintained at higher level. By contrast, in those experiments carried out at later chick stage, parents may have acquired the capacity for offspring recognition through association with their offspring, resulting in the decrease of their probability accepting unrelated young.

    The results of the second nest duplication experiment revealed a complete rejection of their own offspring by parents, while simultaneously accepting unrelated cross-fostered young (Fig. 3D), providing further support for the hypothesis that early parent−offspring association is crucial for parental capacity in recognizing offspring. Deprivation of parental association with their offspring during the early chick stage led to the establishment of recognition for cross-fostering unrelated young. The significant disparity between the outcomes of the first and second nest duplication experiments (Fig. 3D vs. 3C) emphasizes that parents establish their capacity for offspring recognition no later than day 11 under natural conditions. Integrating temporal variations from regression lines in Fig. 2EF, it appears that parents may start learning offspring phenotypic traits as potential signals for recognition around day eight, but they may require an additional 2–3 days to full acquire the capacity for recognizing their offspring.

    By investigating parental care behaviors in response to experimentally manipulated offspring signals in the Azure-winged Magpie, we demonstrated that parents can progressively acquire the capacity for offspring recognition with offspring growth via early association with their offspring. Any factors that disrupt parent−offspring association or introduce unrelated young into the nest before offspring phenotypic traits fully develop can impact parental capacity for offspring recognition, hence resulting in the occurrence of alternative parenting behaviors in provisioning the offspring.

    Lifang Gao: Writing – original draft, Methodology, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. Wen Zhang: Methodology, Investigation. Wenjing Zhu: Methodology, Investigation. Yichen Wu: Methodology, Investigation. Ran Xu: Methodology, Investigation. Ningning Sun: Methodology, Investigation, Conceptualization. Yujie Wang: Investigation, Methodology. Biyun Jia: Methodology, Investigation. Bo Du: Supervision, Methodology, Investigation, Funding acquisition, Resources, Writing – review & editing.

    The ethical permits for life history data collection and individual sampling were issued by the Ethical Committee of School of Life Sciences, Lanzhou University (EAF2020002, EAF2021020). All works in the field were carried out under the Wildlife Conservation Law of the Thirteenth National People’s Congress of China (20170101, 20181026).

    Datasets supporting the statistical analyses can be accessed from the Dryad Digital Repository, https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6q573n630.

    The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

    Authors will thank Qingmiao Ren, Xinwei Da, Guoliang Chen, Lili Xian, Juanjuan Luo, Haiyang Zhang, Fangyuan Luo, and Qian Wang for their assistance in the collection of life history data in the past eight years.

  • Akkaynak, D., Stoddard, M.C., 2023. Common cuckoos do not mimic the size and shape of host eggs. Behav. Ecol. 34, 804–815.
    Becciu, P., Campioni, L., Massa, B., dell’Omo, G., 2021. Unconditional adoption rules out the need for parent–offspring recognition in a single-brooded colonial seabird. Ethology 127, 605–612.
    Beecher, M.D., Beecher, I.M., Hahn, S., 1981. Parent-offspring recognition in bank swallows (Riparia riparia): Ⅱ. Development and acoustic basis. Anim. Behav. 29, 95–101.
    Bouwman, K.M., Lessells, C.M., Komdeur, J., 2005. Male Reed Buntings do not adjust parental effort in relation to extrapair paternity. Behav. Ecol. 16, 499–506.
    Cherry, M.I., Bennett, T.D., 2001. Egg colour mataching in an African cuckoo, as revealed by ultraviolet-visible reflectance spectrophotometry. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 565–571.
    Da, X.W., Xian, L.L., Luo, J.J., Gao, L.F., Du, B., 2018. Azure-winged Magpies Cyanopica cyanus trade off reproductive success and parental care by establishing a size hierarchy among nestlings. Ibis 160, 769–778.
    Dowling, J.L., Diane, C.N., Webster, M.S., 2016. Kin signatures learned in the egg? Red-backed fairy-wren songs are similar to their mother’s in-nest calls and songs. Front. Ecol. Evol. 4, 1–9.
    Du, B., Guan, M.M., Ren, Q.M., Chen, G.L., 2015. Cuckolded male ground tits increase parental care for the brood. Anim. Behav. 110, 61–67.
    Forbes, L.S., Grosshans, R., Glassey, B., 2002. Multiple incentives for parental optimism and brood reduction in blackbirds. Ecology 83, 2529–2541.
    Gao, L.F., Zhang, H.Y., Zhang, W., Sun, Y.H., Liang, M.J., Du, B., 2020. Effects of extra-pair paternity and maternity on the provisioning strategies of the azure-winged magpie Cyanopica cyanus. Ibis 162, 627–636.
    Gao, L.F., Zhang, X.D., Yang, T.C., Wang, S.X., Wang, Q., Liu, F.Y., et al., 2022. Conspecific nest-raiding directs more at dominant breeders in the azure-winged magpie. Curr. Zool. 6, 734–736.
    García-Vigón, E., Veiga, J.P., Cordero, P.J., 2009. Male feeding rate and extrapair paternity in the facultatively polygynous spotless starling. Anim. Behav. 78, 1335–1341.
    Griffith, S.C., Owens, I.P., Thuman, K.A., 2022. Extra pair paternity in birds: a review of interspecific variation and adaptive function. Mol. Ecol. 11, 2195–2212.
    Hamilton, W.D., 1964. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Ⅰ and Ⅱ. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–52.
    Johnson, A.E., Freedberg, S., 2014. Variable facial plumage in juvenile Cliff Swallows: a potential offspring recognition cue? Auk 131, 121–128.
    Jovanovic, T., Megna, N.L., Maestripieri, D., 2000. Early maternal recognition of offspring vocalizations in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Primates 41, 421–428.
    Lefevre, K., Montgomerie, R., Gaston, A.J., 1998. Parent–offspring recognition in thick-billed murres (Aves: Alcidae). Anim. Behav. 55, 925–938.
    Li, Q., Zhang, L., 2010. Parent–offspring recognition in Brandt’s voles, Lasiopodomys brandti. Anim. Behav. 79, 797–801.
    Luther, D.A., 2008. Signaller: receiver coordination and the timing of communication in Amazonian birds. Biol. Lett. 4, 651–654.
    Mak, G.K., Weiss, S., 2010. Paternal recognition of adult offspring mediated by newly generated CNS neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 753–758.
    Malmkvist, J., 2019. Early recognition of offspring vocalisation by mink mothers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 212, 109–113.
    Martin, T.E., 1995. Avian life history evolution in relation to nest sites, nest predation, and food. Ecol. Monogr. 65, 101–127.
    Mateo, J.M., 2017. The ontogeny of kin-recognition mechanisms in Belding’s ground squirrels. Physiol. Behav. 173, 279–284.
    Møller, A.P., Birkhead, T.R., 1993. Certainty of paternity covaries with paternal care in birds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33, 261–268.
    Müller, J.K., Eggert, A.K., 1990. Time-dependent shifts between infanticidal and parental behavior in female burying beetles a mechanism of indirect mother-offspring recognition. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 27, 11–16.
    Perlut, N.G., Kelly, L.M., Zalik, N.J., Strong, A.M., 2012. Male Savannah Sparrows provide less parental care with increasing paternity loss. Northeast. Nat. 9, 335–344.
    Pitcher, B.J., Charrier, I., Harcourt, R.G., 2015. Chemical fingerprints reveal clues to identity, heterozygosity, and relatedness. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 11146–11147.
    Ren, Q.M., Luo, S., Du, X.J., Chen, G.L., Song, S., Du, B., 2016. Helper effects in the azure-winged magpie Cyanopica cyana in relation to highly-clumped nesting pattern and highest frequency of conspecific nest-raiding. J. Avian Biol. 47, 449–456.
    Royle, N.J., Smiseth, P.T., Kölliker, M., 2012. The Evolution of Parental Care. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
    Smith, M.G., Kaiser, S.A., Scott, S.T., Webster, M.S., 2018. Variation in nest characteristics and brooding patterns of female black-throated blue warblers is associated with thermal cues. Auk 135, 733–747.
    Soler, J.J., Aviles, J.M., Soler, M., Møller, A.P., 2003. Evolution of host egg mimicry in a brood parasite, the great spotted cuckoo. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 79, 551–563.
    Sorenson, M.D., Payne, R.B., 2002. Molecular genetic perspectives on avian brood parasitism. Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 388–400.
    Stoleson, S.H., Beissinger, S.R., 1997. Hatching asynchrony, brood reduction, and food limitation in a neotropical parrot. Ecol. Monogr. 67, 131–154.
    Trivers, R.L., 1974. Parent–offspring conflict. Am. Zool. 14, 247–262.
    Zhu, Z.Q., Zi, S.M., Gao, L.F., Zhang, X.D., Liu, F.Y., Wang, Q., et al., 2023. A diagnosis model of parental care: how parents optimize their provisioning strategy in brood reduction? Curr. Zool. 69, 385–392.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(4)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (5) PDF downloads (0) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return