Kevin A. Wood, Kane Brides, Maurice E. Durham, Richard D. Hearn. 2021: Adults have more male-biased sex ratios than first-winter juveniles in wintering duck populations. Avian Research, 12(1): 51. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-021-00286-1
Citation: Kevin A. Wood, Kane Brides, Maurice E. Durham, Richard D. Hearn. 2021: Adults have more male-biased sex ratios than first-winter juveniles in wintering duck populations. Avian Research, 12(1): 51. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-021-00286-1

Adults have more male-biased sex ratios than first-winter juveniles in wintering duck populations

Funds: 

the Wildfowl & Wetlands Trust 

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Kevin A. Wood, kevin.wood@wwt.org.uk

  • Received Date: 26 May 2021
  • Accepted Date: 26 Sep 2021
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 04 Oct 2021
  • Background 

    The long-term monitoring of demographic changes in waterbird populations remains limited, but such information can be valuable for conservationists and waterbird managers. Biased sex ratios can indicate differences in survival rates between sexes. In particular, differences in the sex ratios of fledged juveniles and adults can provide insight into the development of male bias among populations.

    Methods 

    In this study, we used data from individual birds captured over a 57-year period to assess the extent, and temporal variability in male bias in nine populations of ducks wintering in the United Kingdom: Gadwall (Mareca strepera), Northern Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos), Northern Pintail (Anas acuta), Common Pochard (Aythya ferina), Common Shelduck (Tadorna tadorna), Northern Shoveler (Spatula clypeata), Eurasian Teal (Anas crecca), Tufted Duck (Aythya fuligula), and Eurasian Wigeon (Mareca penelope).

    Results 

    Overall, eight of these populations were significantly male-biased and adults were more male-biased than first-winter juveniles for all nine populations. The increased male bias among adults is consistent with the hypothesis that factors such as higher mortality of reproductive-age females during the breeding season is a major cause of male bias in duck populations. However, such predation cannot explain the male bias detected in first-winter juveniles in four of the populations. The temporal trends in male bias differed between adults and first-winter juveniles in Northern Mallard, Northern Pintail, Common Pochard, Common Shelduck, Eurasian Teal, Tufted Duck, and Eurasian Wigeon. Over the study period we found increased male bias among adult Northern Mallard, Northern Pintail, Common Pochard, Common Shelduck, and Tufted Duck as well as both adult and first-winter juvenile Northern Shoveler.

    Conclusions 

    We provide evidence that among wintering duck populations, sex ratios are typically male-biased, with adults exhibiting stronger male-biased sex ratios than first-winter juveniles. Improved monitoring of sex ratios of wintering waterbirds would help to increase our understanding of changes in waterbird demography, population structure, and observed population trends; our study shows that birds caught during ringing projects can be a valuable source of such data.

  • Behavioural traits have been traditional sources of phylogenetic information along with morphological data (de Queiroz and Wimberger 1993; Kusmierski et al. 1997; Gatesy et al. 1999), even though a behavioural repertoire was often believed to be highly adaptive. Currently, behavioural data together with morphological data are being replaced by much more extensive molecular data, which exhibit lower rates of homoplasy and better tree resolution due to a higher number of informative characters (Scotland et al. 2003). Additionally, molecular methods have been routinely applicable and have become much cheaper, while the complete description of the behavioural repertoire requires months and years of effort, and the homology of some traits remains uncertain. The use of behavioural data combined with phylogenetic analysis has decreased in recent years (Price et al. 2011). Nevertheless, this combination of behavioural data and phylogenetic analysis allows us to study the evolution of behaviour in a phylogenetic context and thus, in the unprecedented detail the evolution of specific behavioural traits or the whole repertoire (Senter 2008; Duda and Zrzavy 2013; Lister 2014; Ligon et al. 2018; Ericson et al. 2020).

    It is already documented that behaviour often carries significant phylogenetic signal (Prum 1990; de Queiroz and Wimberger 1993; McLennan and Mattern 2001; Rendall and Di Fiore 2007; Scholes III 2008; Miles and Fuxjager 2019), which could allow behavioural traits to be used for supporting particular nodes as well, especially where data subsets produce conflicting support (Gatesy et al. 1999). Nevertheless, there is a strong inequality in majority of the works in the number of inspected behavioural traits compared to morphological or molecular data, as behavioural traits are rarely covered in datasets (Paterson et al. 1995; Kennedy et al. 1996; Blomberg et al. 2003; Ligon et al. 2018). Additionally, behavioural data allow us to reconstruct the ancestral behavioural repertoire for particular groups (Cap et al. 2002; Lister 2014), which is an important task if we are to understand how behaviour has evolved over time with regard to the homoplasy and adaptive potential of behaviour for the particular groups (Lefebvre et al. 2016; Downs et al. 2019; Penndorf and Aplin 2020). The behavioural traits have proven to be at least as good as the morphological data, for example in displays of manakins, which are completely congruent with their syringeal anatomy (Prum 1990). The usefulness of behaviour for phylogeny estimation was also supported by de Queiroz and Wimberger (1993), who combined and analysed multiple datasets against each other and found that morphological data did not prove to be better than behavioural. Similarly, as in the case of five genera in the Gasterosteidae fish family, where both morphological and behavioural data produced the same topology and the total evidence data provided a robust phylogeny (McLennan and Mattern 2001). Same results occurred in the case of toucans and barbets, it was proven that the body size can predict their mating behaviour and vice versa, and that mating behaviour can catalyse diversification (Miles and Fuxjager 2019).

    We identified several promising groups for inspection of the evolution of behaviour in a phylogenetic context and comparison of behavioural traits with other data sets—for example, Anatidae (Lorenz 1941; Johnsgard 1962, 1965; Livezey 1991), Gruidae (for detail see below), Bovidae [e.g. Walther (1984)—for preliminary assessment see Vrba and Schaller (2000); Deleporte and Cap (2014)]. Some datasets have been analysed in pioneering works — e.g. Felidae [Mattern and McLennan (2000), with potential additions of traits from Hemmer (1966); Leyhausen (1979); Sunquist and Sunquist (1996)], Macropodiformes (Ganslosser 1993, 1995); Pipridae (Prum 1990, 1994, 1998)—and could benefit from revising the phylogeny of specific groups associated with the denser taxa sampling and/or more (molecular) data available. Cranes (Gruidae) seemed to be excellent candidates for such detailed inspection of the evolution of behavioural traits in a phylogenetic context, as they represent a small, old group with well-corroborated phylogenetic relationships on the species level, and with a complex and well-described behavioural repertoire.

    Specifically, cranes form a monophyletic group with the deepest separation between Balearicinae and Gruinae estimated at some 31-37 million years ago, with well-supported relationships among particular species (Gaubert et al. 2005; Krajewski et al. 2010; Krajewski 2019). Their closest relatives are the family Aramidae, then the family Psophiidae and the family Rallidae (Prum et al. 2015). Ellis et al. (1998) published a complete homologized sociogram of impressive and complex behaviour. Nevertheless, these unique data have neither been combined with molecular data for the detection of phylogenetic signals in a behavioural repertoire nor analysed for the evolution of specific behaviour traits under phylogenetic control.

    Cranes are tall, omnivorous birds inhabiting wetlands and grasslands worldwide, except for Antarctica and South America, and number only 15 species (Johnsgard 1983). Eleven crane species are threatened with extinction, according to the IUCN Red list, including one species that is critically endangered, three species that are endangered, and six species that are vulnerable (Harris and Mirande 2013; IUCN 2021). Some of them migrate seasonally over great distances (e.g. Grus grus), some migrate only altitudinally (e.g. Grus paradisea), and some do not migrate (e.g. Balearica spp.), in conformity with ecological parameters of the inhabited regions (Johnsgard 1983; Prange 2016). All cranes are monogamous with stable pairs and lay a small clutch of no more than three eggs (Johnsgard 1983). The pairs are created and maintained through complex dances, accompanied by calls performed by both partners (Johnsgard 1983). Frequency of pair dances and thus harmonization of the pair affects their reproductive success (Takeda et al. 2019). Cranes also exhibit a system of social postures, displays, and other activities as feeding habits or alerts, made up of at least 60 behavioural characters (Ellis et al. 1998; Panov et al. 2010). Dances are usually completed with a vocalization. Archibald (1976) assumed phylogeny of cranes based on unison calls of different species and succeeded in dividing cranes into subfamilies Balearicinae and Gruinae and proposed three genera: Anthropoides, Grus, and Bugeranus. Besides pair dances, even unpaired subadults exhibit dancing behaviour. It is considered that dancing facilitates socialization and pair formation as displacement activity or play behaviour (Dinets 2013). Furthermore, certain displays may be related to dominance as performed by singletons in wintering flocks (Takeda et al. 2018). The closest relative of the family Gruidae is the Limpkin (Aramus guarauna), which is a snail specialist living in the wetlands of Florida, the Caribbean, and Central and South America (Fain et al. 2007). The behavioural repertoire of Limpkin is not well explored, although it is assumed that they show some crane-like displays (Bryan and Kirwan 2018). For the other outgroup, Purple Swamphen (Porphyrio porphyrio) was chosen. It belongs to the family Rallidae, which is sister clade to the Limpkin and crane lineages (Gong et al. 2017).

    In this study we attempt to inspect the evolution of cranes in a phylogenetic context, using these approaches: (1) reconstruction of ancestral behavioural repertoire of all cranes and their particular groups and the identification of behavioural evolutionary novelties; (2) estimation of phylogenetic informativeness and phylogenetic signal in the behavioural repertoire of cranes using the phylogenetic control based on mitochondrial genomes; (3) comparison of the influence of various scorings of behavioural traits on reconstruction of ancestral behavioural repertoire and its phylogenetic informativeness.

    In this study, all 15 species of cranes recognised by all latest reviews (e.g. Johnsgard 1983; Harris and Mirande 2013; Prange 2016) were analysed (see names in Fig. 1, or in Additional file 1: Table S1). We used a complete sociogram from the study of Ellis et al. (1998) in which all behavioural traits observed in the cranes are specified and scored. The complete sociogram contains 107 behavioural traits, including vocalization (17 characters), agonistic displays (32 characters), and parental behaviour (13 characters). As some behavioural traits are exhibited by all species or by only one species, the total number of possibly phylogenetically informative characters was 28, composed of vocalization (2 characters), agonistic behaviour (23 characters), and pair-related behaviour (3 characters). Names of all inspected specific behavioural traits and their abbreviations are specified in Table 1; for a compressed description of individual traits, see Additional file 1: Table S1; the original data are presented in Ellis et al. (1998). The original scoring from the sociogram was accepted and transformed into a numerical matrix in this way: 0=absent, 1=occurring occasionally, 2=occurring regularly, 3=occurring often, 4=occurring very often, NA=undocumented. We also created a binary version of the matrix containing only information about the presence/absence of inspected traits (Additional file 1: Table S2).

    Figure  1.  Distribution of behavioural traits with strong phylogenetic signal across the phylogeny. The phylogenetic tree contains numbers of recognized nodes. Traits are presented by letter codes (for full names see Table 1). Codes for multi-state characters: 0=absent, 1=occurring occasionally, 2=occurring regularly, 3=occurring often, 4=occurring very often, crossed white area=undocumented
    Table  1.  List of studied traits with their code names, Pagel's lambda and Blomberg's K values and consistency index for both multi-state (λ) and binary (B-λ) data
    Display Code name λ B-λ K B-K CI B-CI
    Vocalizations
    Location call LC 0.99 0.51 0.5
    Copulatory call CC 0.99 0.48 0.61 0.61 1 1
    Agonistic displays
    Iris expansion IE 0.99 0.99 0.30 0.30 0.5 0.5
    Feather tuft erection FTE 6×e-5 0.37 0.43
    Gular expansion GE 0.99 0.99 0.48 0.55 0.67 0.5
    Head rub HR 0.99 1.31 0.75
    Tertial elevation TE 0.87 5×e-5 0.57 0.64 0.5 0.5
    Pre strut PS 0.99 0.61 1
    Strut S 0.99 0.62 0.5
    Head flick HF 0.99 0.99 0.73 0.57 0.6 0.33
    Dorsal preen DP 0.99 0.32 0.67
    Ventral preen VP 0.99 0.99 0.35 0.35 1 1
    Bill down hold BDH 6×e-5 0.26 0.67
    Bill down growl BDG 0.99 0.99 0.40 0.58 1 1
    Bill down sweep BDS 0.99 7×e-05 0.45 0.49 0.5 0.33
    Blow bubbles BB 0.99 0.99 0.24 0.33 0.67 0.5
    Head lower ruffle HLR 0.99 0.99 0.81 0.64 0.67 0.5
    Ruffle bow RB 0.99 6×e-05 0.63 0.64 0.75 0.5
    Wing spread hold WSH 0.99 0.99 0.41 0.60 0.67 0.5
    Wing spread flap WSF 6×e-5 6×e-05 0.54 0.47 0.33 0.33
    Tail wag TW 0.99 0.99 0.81 0.64 0.67 0.5
    Catapult C 0.99 0.99 0.80 1.39 1 1
    Butterfly B 0.99 0.99 0.93 0.96 1 1
    Hoover or neck crane HNC1 0.99 0.99 1.92 3.28 0.67 1
    Crouch CR 0.87 0.33 0.4
    Pair related behaviour
    Run flap glide RFG 0.99 0.99 0.35 0.35 1 1
    Hoover or neck crane HNC2 0.99 0.99 3.07 3.07 0.5 1
    Bill stab BS 0.99 0.99 0.63 0.35 0.75 1
    Significant values are in underlined italics. Non-underlined italics are the values which are less similar than would be assumed under Brownian motion. For the calculation of Blomberg's K Brownian motion model was selected for all the traits
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    To control behavioural traits for phylogeny, we used complete sequences of mitochondrial genomes of all 15 species of cranes (Krajewski et al. 2010) and available mitochondrial sequences for the Limpkin (Boast et al. 2019) and the Purple Swamphen which we used as the outgroups (Fain et al. 2007; García-R et al. 2014). Currently, the mitochondrial genome is the only genetic data source available for all crane species [Krajewski (2019); our inspection of GenBank database]. All the sequences were gathered from GenBank (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/; table of access numbers of analysed species is available in Additional file 1: Table S3).

    Sequences were aligned in Geneious? 10.1.3 (2005-2017 Biomatters Ltd.) using global alignment. The best-fitting substitution model of DNA sequence evolution—the Tamura-Nei 1993 model—was selected by MEGA X [ver. 10.0.5; Kumar et al. (2018)] under the Akaike Information Criterion. Maximum likelihood and Bootstrap analyses with 10, 000 replications were also conducted in MEGA X [ver. 10.0.5; Kumar et al. (2018)]. Since the topology of the phylogenetic tree of cranes was identical to the time-calibrated tree of Krajewski et al.(2010, 2019), we used the branch lengths from Krajewski et al. (2010). Phylogenetic tree of cranes based strictly on molecular data was then imported in nexus format and paired with the matrix of behavioural traits into Mesquite ver. 3.40. History of characters was traced using likelihood methods. The same tree with branch lengths created in MEGA X was also exported into R version 3.5.1 (?2009-2018 RStudio, Inc). The strength of the phylogenetic signal of individual traits was evaluated by calculation of Pagel's lambda, which allows a more complex model of evolution with strong (lambda=1) to weak (lambda=0) phylogenetic covariation, and Blomberg's K, which is a widely used metric to test the phylogenetic signal, using the "phylosig" function in the phylosignal package (Keck et al. 2016). Blomberg's K allows us to evaluate a strong phylogenetic signal (K > 1) as well as non-random diversification of trait (K=0), or whether a trait evolved according to the Brownian model of evolution (K=1), which means that trait evolved independently on evolution. Additionally, all the results were tested if statistically significant (p < 0.05). Behavioural traits were visualized on the tree by the "phylo.heatmap" function in the phytools package (Revell 2012), while the synapomorphies were mapped over the tree using the Map Characters function in the WinClada program, ver. 1.00.08 (K. Nixon?1999-2002), which was used for basic assessment of informativeness (consistency index, hereafter abbreviated as CI) of the behavioural trait subset too [also using NONA version 2.0; Goloboff (1999)]. This package was used as well to compare behavioural traits with morphological characters (cf. de Queiroz and Wimberger 1993), using the matrix compiled by Livezey (1998; specifically using his matrix from Appendix 2). The same comparisons were performed with a multistate matrix as well as with a binary matrix.

    Firstly, all 28 possibly phylogenetically informative behavioural characters alone cannot be used for resolving species-level relationships within the Gruidae. Specifically, the strict consensus and bootstrap methods detected no supported clade within cranes. Only the majority consensus identified these six clades with stronger support: both Balearica species are grouped together in 94% of cases, Bugeranus with Antropoides with 86%, and both these groups form one major clade with 70% support. The second major clade with 58% support consists of Antigone canadensis, Leucogeranus and all species of the genus Grus. Inside the last group with the 78% support Grus japonensis stands aside, while other Grus species form its sister group with 89% support. From these relationships only several subgroups are concordant with the topology based on mitochondrial genome—the subgroup comprising Balearica species, Bugeranus with Antropoides, and all species of the genus Grus (with G. japonensis sister to other Grus species) respectively.

    In contrast, 48 possibly phylogenetically informative morphological characters provide higher resolution, specifically the support for Balearica, Anthropoides and Antigone vipio-rubicunda-antigone clades under the strict consensus and these groups plus Gruinae and Leucogeranus-Antigone-Grus clades under the bootstrap. The majority consensus has a full resolution identical with the original study (Fig. 5 in Livezey 1998).

    Moreover, behavioural traits under phylogenetic control exhibited predominantly (82%) a strong phylogenetic signal (Table 1) when applicated on phylogenetic tree based on genetic data. Support values for the phylogenetic tree used in the analyses are listed in Additional file 2: Table S4.

    Pagel's lambda together with Blomberg's K and CI for inspected behavioural traits are specified in Table 1. The values indicate that the evolution of one third of them (e.g. Hoover or neck crane of both types or Head rub) was quite conservative and closely related to phylogeny. Traits mapped on the tree along with node numbers are shown in Fig. 1.

    The reconstruction of specific behavioural trait evolution was predominantly well-resolved across the whole phylogenetic tree. The following text describes the evolution of the traits for the individual nodes. Unique changes in behaviour are displayed in Fig. 2. See detailed likelihoods of individual multi-state trait states in ancestors in Additional file 2: Table S4.

    Node 1: The ancestral crane (Gruidae) appears to exhibit the following behaviour to some extent: Bare-skin-present (BSP), Bare-skin-expansion (BSE), Feather-tuft-erection (FTE), Head-rub (HR), Tertial-elevation (TE), Prestrut (PS), Strut (S), Head-flick (HF), Dorsal-preen (DP), Ventral-preen (VP), Bill-down-hold (BDH), Bill-down-growl (BDG), Bill-down-sweep (BDS), Blow-bubbles (BB), Head-lower-ruffle (HLR), Ruffle-bow (RB), Wing-spread-flap (WSF), Tail-wag (TW), Hoover or neck-crane of both types (HNC1 and HNC2), Crouch (CR) Run-flap-glide (RFG), Bill-stab (BS), and Shading (SH).

    Node 3: The ancestor of Gruinae probably exhibited Ventral-preen (VP) and Run-flap-glide (RFG), at the same intensity of performance. Tertial-elevation (TE), Bill-down-growl (BDG) and Bill-stab (BS) in different levels of intensity appear to be typical for those ancestors.

    Node 5: In the common ancestor of three Antigone species (A. antigone, A. rubicunda and A. vipio) Gular-expansion (GE) and Catapult (C) appeared. Catapult is a display presented exclusively in these three species.

    Node 9: The ancestor of genera Bugeranus and Anthropoides probably lost these behavioural traits: Tertial-elevation (TE), and Hoover or neck-crane of both types (HNC1 and HNC2). On the contrary, Head-lower-ruffle (HLR), Wing-spread-hold (WSP), and Tail-wag (TW) likely re-appeared in this group.

    Node 11: All Grus species perform exclusively Copulatory call (CC). Only species of genus Grus except G. japonensis perform Butterfly (B).

    Figure  2.  Crane phylogeny based on complete mtDNA with mapped non-homoplasious (black) and homoplasious (white) a changes of multi-state behavioural characters. Traits are presented by letter codes (for full names see Table 1). Codes for multi-state characters: 0=absent, 1=occurring occasionally, 2=occurring regularly, 3=occurring often, 4=occurring very often. Arrows indicate direction of change in specific trait (e.g. 1 to > 2 means that trait changed in given node from occurring occasionally to occurring regularly)

    In summary, ancestral cranes already exhibited quite a rich behavioural repertoire, including sophisticated epigamic behaviour, and only a minority of the traits changed during subsequent evolution.

    General features of phylogenetic signal and ancestral reconstruction of specific behavioural traits under the binary scoring were similar to the results obtained using the multistate coding; strong phylogenetic signal was detected in a lesser amount, i.e. 71% of traits (Table 1). Number of changes, Pagel's lambda together with Blomberg's K and CI for inspected behavioural traits are specified in Table 1. The evolution of traits is displayed in Fig. 3 and the traits are also mapped on the tree in Fig. 1. The reconstruction of ancestral behaviour repertoire detected these associations; see detailed likelihoods of individual traits coded binary on particular nodes in Additional file 3: Table S5.

    Node 4: Bill-down-sweep (BDS) disappeared in the genera Antigone, Anthropoides, Bugeranus and Grus.

    Node 6: Catapult (C) and Gular-expansion appeared in genus Antigone, except for A. canadensis.

    Node 9: Tertial-elevation (TE), Ruffle-bow (RB), and Hoover or neck-crane of both types (HNC1 and HNC2) disappeared. Head-lower-ruffle (HLR), Wing-spread-hold (WSH) and Tail-wag (TW) appeared in genera Bugeranus and Anthropoides.

    Node 11: Copulatory call (CC) evolved in genus Grus.

    Node 12: Butterfly (B) posture appeared in genus Grus, except for G. japonensis.

    Figure  3.  Crane phylogeny based on complete mtDNA with mapped non-homoplasious (black) and homoplasious (white) changes of binary behavioural characters. Traits are presented by letter codes (for full names see Table 1). Codes for binary characters: 0=absent, 1=present

    The Balearicinae did not obtain or lose any exclusive trait, but the traits in Gruinae changed as the subfamily diversified. Contrary to the multi-state data matrix, the most changes happened in the genera Bugeranus and Anthropoides.

    Mean CI value for behavioural traits was 0.69 for multistate and 0.70 for binary variant. Mean CI value for morphological matrix under the same mitochondrial phylogenetic control 0.75 (binary version was not performed due the nature of traits) shows no significant difference to mean CI obtained for the multistate variant of behavioural traits (t-test, p=0.36).

    The present behavioural matrix with 15 crane species and 28 behavioural traits is compatible by size with many studies reviewed by de Queiroz and Wimberger (1993) in their inspection of usefulness of behaviour for phylogeny estimation. The detected level of homoplasy (mean CI value 0.69 for multistate and 0.70 for binary variants) in the behavioural traits of cranes inspected in this study seems to be rather lower than values (mean CI value 0.84) for similar data sets reviewed by de Queiroz and Wimberger (1993), but otherwise fully within the range of reviewed studies [CI values from 0.62 to 1; de Queiroz and Wimberger (1993)]. The level of homoplasy in inspected behavioural data is a little bit lower than in morphological data (CI 0.75), but there is no significant difference between them. Behavioural repertoire of cranes itself is not able to provide robust and reliable phylogenetic relationships, probably because of small number of phylogenetically informative traits. From all 107 identified behavioural traits (Ellis et al. 1998), only 26% (28 characters) is possibly phylogenetically informative. For comparison, from 570 morphological characters scored by Livezey (1998) only 8% (48 characters) is potentially phylogenetically informative. Nevertheless, these morphological characters provide quite robust phylogenetic relationships, which exhibit some congruence with relationships detected based on genetic data (compare Fig. 5 in Livezey 1998 with Fig. 2.1 in Krajewski 2019).

    Nevertheless, in combination with genetic data, behavioural traits exhibit strong phylogenetic signal. Our findings correspond with other works according to which behavioural data carry strong phylogenetic signal and are no more homoplasious than other data sets. For example, as shown in the meta-analysis of de Queiroz and Wimberger (1993), studies of evolution on bovid behaviour (Deleporte and Cap 2014), behaviour and vocalization in deer (Cap et al. 2002, 2008), grooming behaviour in rodents (Malange et al. 2013) or social displays of the Pelecaniformes (Kennedy et al. 1996), as well as of courtship behaviour in salamanders (Arnold et al. 2017) and in seabird behaviour (Paterson et al. 1995).

    The species-specific behavioural traits, albeit phylogenetically useless, might represent a type of reproductive isolation mechanism to minimize the risk of hybridization between sister or distantly related species (McCarthy 2006). This aspect may be especially relevant for the different genera of cranes capable of hybridization, under captivity conditions as well in the wild (McCarthy 2006; Ottenburghs et al. 2015).

    Reconstruction of crane ancestral behavioural repertoire indicates that complex behavioural repertoire was already present before group began to diversify. After start of diversification change of behavioural repertoire significantly slowed down with the tendency to lose traits rather than evolve new ones, e.g. Bugeranus and Anthropoides species. Even though the Balearicinae separated from Gruinae before approximately 31 Ma (Krajewski et al. 2010; Krajewski 2019), they exhibit no special displays or even unique changes in intensity of traits. A similar pattern was recognized by Johnsgard (1983), who argued that many morphological traits present in Balearicinae are primitive for Gruidae, with Gruinae showing derived modifications. The diversification itself is imbalanced between both sister subfamilies (Krajewski et al. 2010), but that topic is itself beyond the scope of this contribution.

    The most basal species of Gruinae, the Siberian Crane (Leucogeranus leucogeranus), shows the most different repertoire from all other species. However, differences are mainly in the intensity of performed display. This species otherwise exhibits biological parameters very similar to those of other crane species, except for its territorial behaviour, which is exhibited even during wintering (Johnsgard 1983).

    Bugeranus and Anthropoides species as a group exhibit the most changes in behavioural repertoire both in evolution/disappearing traits and changes in frequency of performances. We are not able to explain why in this lineage the disappearing of traits occurs more often than evolution of new ones. It could be expected that the loss of traits is easier rather than de novo evolution, but in any case, it is known that the evolutionary loss might decrease costs under some selection regimes or at least might represent a nonadaptive evolutionary change (e.g. Aragón et al 1999; Beauchamp 1999; Ekman and Ericson 2006).

    The Sandhill Crane (Antigone canadensis) exhibits more similarities in behavioural displays with genus Grus than with genus Antigone. This fact can imply a faster diversification within the rest of Antigone, perhaps due to dynamic demographic changes with much more regular gene flow contacts among populations of A. canadensis during the late Cenozoic (e.g. Wood and Krajewski 1996; Rhymer et al. 2001; Jones et al. 2005). Conversely, this similarity could be a result of ancient hybridization events [as proved e.g. in felids (Li et al. 2016) or in bovines (Wu et al. 2018) by genomic surveys].

    Since the character coding is an important issue in phylogenetic analyses (e.g. Scotland and Pennington 2000), we inspected this aspect too. The ancestral behavioural repertoires exhibited quite different scenarios to binary or multistate coding, in accordance with the results of Sansom and Wills (2013), which demonstrated that different scoring of traits could have a significant effect on results and reconstruction. A multi-state data coding approach showed more complex evolution of behaviour in cranes than the binary approach, but this is likely caused by the decrease of variability in behavioural data under the binary approach. Since the basic parameters of the level of homoplasy (CI) and phylogenetic signal were mostly similar in both approaches, we consider both scenarios as quite equivalent alternatives.

    Whereas there are minimum changes in de novo evolving and losing traits, evolution of behavioural traits in significantly more often accompanied by changes in frequency of preformation specific displays. The changes in the intensity of the traits implies that some of them evolved more rapidly, which could also represent a reproductive isolation mechanism preventing hybridization (Hendry et al. 2000 see also above) and/or some relaxed "phenotype" in newly occupied geographic regions [cf. dispersal phenotype sensu Geist (1987)].

    In future research it might be also useful to compare wider spectrum of different statistical approaches [e.g. maximum parsimony versus Bayesian phylogenetic inference using Lewis's Mk model; Schrago et al. (2018), Spade (2020), Varga et al. (2020), see also Goloboff et al. (2019)] to identify the best supported evolution of behavioural traits.

    Our results suggest that behaviour reflects phylogeny in cranes. The behavioural data of living species gives us the opportunity to decipher evolutionary history of crane in promising details. Considering the well-compiled morphological, behavioural and molecular datasets for cranes (Ellis et al. 1998; Livezey 1998; Krajewski et al. 2010; Panov et al. 2010; Krajewski 2019) the complete sociogram of the Limpkin is the most important facet for the final understanding of the evolution of behaviour cranes and their closest relatives.

    The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-021-00275-4.

    Additional file 1: Table S1. Compressed description of used behavioural traits. Table S2. Behavioural traits. Table S3. GenBank access numbers of analysed species. Figure S1. Node support values for the phylogenetic tree.

    Additional file 2: Table S4. Likelihoods reconstructions of individual traits for particular nodes based on multi-state matrix.

    Additional file 3: Table S5. Likelihoods reconstructions of individual traits for particular nodes based on binary matrix.

    We are very grateful to Carey Krajewski for helpful advice on the mitochondrial data of cranes, Pavel Duda for valuable discussions of methods and results, Anton Baer for English editing, and the editor and two anonymous reviewers for very valuable comments which improved the manuscript significantly.

    NN and JR conceived the idea; NN created a data matrix from existing data sources, performed the analysis and led the writing. Both JR and NN contributed significantly to the final version. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    This work was financially supported by the grant agency of the University of South Bohemia (04-048/2019/P).

    The data matrix and access numbers to genetic material on GenBank are available in additional files; other data subsets used and/or analysed during this study are available from the corresponding author on request.

    Not applicable.

    Not applicable.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

  • Aebischer NJ. Fifty-year trends in UK hunting bags of birds and mammals, and calibrated estimation of national bag size, using GWCT's National Gamebag Census. Eur J Wildl Res. 2019;65: 64.
    Albrecht T, Hořák D, Kreisinger J, Weidinger K, Klvaňa P, Michot TC. Factors determining pochard nest predation along a wetland gradient. J Wildlife Manag. 2006;70: 784–91.
    Alexander WC. Differential sex distributions of wintering diving ducks (Aythyini) in North America. Am Birds. 1983;37: 26–9.
    Arnold TW. Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike's Information Criterion. J Wildlife Manag. 2010;74: 1175–8.
    Bartoń K. MuMIn: Model selection and model averaging based on information criteria. R package version 1.13.4. 2012. .
    Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Software. 2015;67: 1–48.
    Bellebaum J, Mädlow W. Survival explains sex ratio in an introduced Mandarin Duck Aix galericulata population. Ardea. 2015;103: 183–7.
    Bellrose FC, Scott TG, Hawkins AS, Low JB. Sex ratios and age ratios in North American ducks. Ill Nat Hist Surv Bull. 1961;27: 391–486.
    Blums P, Mednis A. Secondary sex ratio in Anatinae. Auk. 1996;113: 505–11.
    Bolen EG. Sex ratios in the Black-bellied Tree Duck. J Wildlife Manag. 1970;34: 68–73.
    Brides K, Wood KA, Hearn RD, Fijen TPM. Changes in the sex ratio of the Common Pochard Aythya ferina in Europe and North Africa. Wildfowl. 2017;67: 100–12.
    Burnham KP, Anderson DR, Huyvaert KP. AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65: 23–35.
    Carbone C, Owen M. Differential migration of the sexes of Pochard Aythya ferina: results from a European survey. Wildfowl. 1995;46: 99–108.
    Carney SM. Species, age and sex identification of ducks using wing plumage. Washington: US Department of the Interior, US Fish and Wildlife Service; 1992.
    Choudhury S, Black JM. Testing the behavioural dominance and dispersal hypothesis in Pochard. Ornis Scand. 1991;22: 155–9.
    Christensen TK, Fox AD. Changes in age and sex ratios amongst samples of hunter-shot wings from common duck species in Denmark 1982–2010. Eur J Wildl Res. 2014;60: 303–12.
    Clutton-Brock TH. Sex-ratio variation in birds. Ibis. 1986;128: 317–29.
    Cooper N, Bond AL, Davis JL, Portela Miguez R, Tomsett L, Helgen KM. Sex biases in bird and mammal natural history collections. Proc R Soc B. 2019;286: 20192025.
    Cox DR, Snell EJ. Analysis of binary data. 2nd ed. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall; 1989.
    Crawley MJ. Statistics: an introduction using R. Chichester: Wiley; 2005.
    Dean WRJ, Skead DM. The sex ratio in Yellowbilled Duck, Redbilled Teal and Southern Pochard. Ostrich. 1977;48: 82–5.
    Donald PF. Adult sex ratios in wild bird populations. Ibis. 2007;149: 671–92.
    Dormann CF, Elith J, Bacher S, Buchmann C, Carl G, Carré G, et al. Collinearity: a review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography. 2013;36: 27–46.
    Eberhart-Phillips LJ, Küpper C, Carmona-Isunza MC, Vincze O, Zefania S, Cruz-López M, et al. Demographic causes of adult sex ratio variation and their consequences for parental cooperation. Nat Commun. 2018;9: 1651.
    Evans DM, Day KR. Migration patterns and sex ratios of diving ducks wintering in Northern Ireland with specific reference to Lough Neagh. Ringing Migr. 2001;20: 358–63.
    Fox AD, Cristensen TK. Could falling female sex ratios among first-winter northwest European duck populations contribute to skewed adult sex ratios and overall population declines? Ibis. 2018;160: 929–35.
    Fox AD, King R, Watkin J. Seasonal variation in weight, body measurements and condition of free-living Teal. Bird Study. 1992;39: 53–62.
    Frew RT, Brides K, Clare T, Maclean L, Rigby D, Tomlinson CT, et al. Temporal changes in the sex ratio of the Common Pochard Aythya ferina compared to four other duck species at Martin Mere, Lancashire, UK. Wildfowl. 2018;68: 140–54.
    Guillemain M, Fox AD, Pöysä H, Väänänen V-M, Christensen TK, Triplet P, et al. Autumn survival inferred from wing age ratios: Wigeon juvenile survival half that of adults at best? J Ornithol. 2013;154: 351–8.
    Hommel G. A stagewise rejective multiple test procedure based on a modified Bonferroni test. Biometrika. 1988;75: 383–6.
    Johnson DH, Sargeant AB. Impact of red fox predation on the sex ratio of prairie mallards. Washington: Department of the Interior, Fish and Wildlife Service; 1977.
    Kear J, Hulme M. Ducks, geese and swans. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
    Ketterson ED, Nolan V Jr. Geographic variation and its climatic correlates in the sex ratio of eastern-wintering Dark-eyed Juncos (Junco hyemalis hyemalis). Ecology. 1976;57: 679–93.
    Korschgen CE. Breeding stress of female eiders in Maine. J Wildl Manag. 1977;41: 360–73.
    Lehikoinen A, Christensen TK, Öst M, Kilpi M, Saurola P, Vattulainen A. Large-scale change in the sex ratio of a declining eider Somateria mollissima population. Wildlife Biol. 2008a;14: 288–301.
    Lehikoinen A, Öst M, Hollmén T, Kilpi M. Does sex-specific duckling mortality contribute to male bias in adult common eiders. Condor. 2008b;110: 574–8.
    Leopold A. Game management. Madison: The University of Wisconsin Press; 1933.
    Madge S, Burn H. Wildfowl: an identification guide to the ducks, geese and swans of the world. London: Christopher Helm; 1988.
    Mayr E. The sex ratio in wild birds. Am Nat. 1939;73: 156–79.
    Metz KJ, Ankney CD. Are brightly coloured male ducks selectively shot by duck hunters? Can J Zool. 1991;69: 279–82.
    Mitchell C, Ogilvie M. Fifty years of wildfowl ringing by The Wildfowl & Wetlands Trust. Wildfowl. 1996;47: 240–7.
    Mitchell C, Fox AD, Harradine J, Clausager I. Measures of annual breeding success amongst Eurasian Wigeon Anas penelope. Bird Study. 2008;55: 43–51.
    Nagelkerke NJD. A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika. 1991;78: 691–2.
    Opermanis O, Mednis A, Bauga I. Duck nests and predators: interaction, specialisation and possible management. Wildlife Biol. 2001;7: 87–96.
    Owen M, Cook WA. Variations in body weight, wing length and condition of Mallard Anas platvrhvnchos platyrhynchos and their relationship to environmental changes. J Zool. 1977;183: 377–95.
    Owen M, Dix M. Sex ratios in some common British wintering ducks. Wildfowl. 1986;37: 104–12.
    Owen M, Mitchell C. Movements and migrations of Wigeon Anas penelope wintering in Britain and Ireland. Bird Study. 1988;35: 47–59.
    Owen M, Montgomery S. Body measurements of Mallard caught in Britain. Wildfowl. 1978;29: 123–34.
    Perdeck AC, Cavé AJ. Sexual differences in migration and winter quarters of ducks ringed in the Netherlands. Wildfowl. 1983;34: 137–43.
    Pöysä H, Linkola P, Paasivaara A. Breeding sex ratios in two declining diving duck species: between-year variation and changes over six decades. J Ornithol. 2019;160: 1015–23.
    R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing [3.6.3]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020. .
    Ramula S, Öst M, Lindén A, Karell P, Kilpi M. Increased male bias in eider ducks can be explained by sex-specific survival of prime-age breeders. PLoS One. 2018;13: e0195415.
    Scott P. The decoy. Severn Wildfowl Trust Ann Rep. 1949;1: 57–61.
    Sheldon BC. Recent studies of avian sex ratios. Heredity. 1998;80: 397–402.
    Signorell A. DescTools: tools for descriptive statistics. R package version 0.99.37. 2020. .
    Sun YH, Bridgman CL, Wu HL, Lee CF, Liu M, Chiang PJ, et al. Sex ratio and survival of Mandarin Ducks in the Tachia River of central Taiwan. Waterbirds. 2011;34: 509–13.
    Székely T, Weissing FJ, Komdeur J. Adult sex ratio variation: implications for breeding system evolution. J Evol Biol. 2014;27: 1500–12.
    Thompson MC, DeLong RL. The use of cannon and rocket-projected nets for trapping shorebirds. Bird-Banding. 1967;38: 214–8.
    Wainwright CB. How to make and use duck traps. Wildfowl. 1957;8: 44–7.
    Wood KA, Ponting J, D'Costa N, Newth JL, Rose PE, Glazov P, et al. Understanding intrinsic and extrinsic drivers of aggressive behaviour in waterbird assemblages: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav. 2017;126: 209–16.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(10)

    1. Jinjie He, Chang Wang, Ying Han, et al. Waterbird community response to wetland and climate changes in the Liaohe River Estuary wetlands, China. Journal of Environmental Management, 2025, 374: 124165. DOI:10.1016/j.jenvman.2025.124165
    2. Rong Ma, Qi Xu, Yongqian Gao, et al. Patterns and drivers of plant sexual systems in the dry-hot valley region of southwestern China. Plant Diversity, 2024, 46(2): 158. DOI:10.1016/j.pld.2023.07.010
    3. Dawei Wu, Taiyu Chen, Wei Hu, et al. Impact of active vegetation restoration and native vegetation on breeding bird communities in coastal wetlands under disturbance by reintroduced Père David’s deer. Global Ecology and Conservation, 2024, 56: e03345. DOI:10.1016/j.gecco.2024.e03345
    4. Lingxuan Gong, Lili Wang, Yao Yao, et al. Integrity and Size of Green Spaces Are Critical for the Functional Diversity of Birds: Evidence from the Eastern and Northwestern Regions of China. Chinese Geographical Science, 2024, 34(6): 1073. DOI:10.1007/s11769-024-1474-1
    5. Xiangyi Li, Zhaojun Li, Yu Chen, et al. Enhancing Bird Diversity in Urban Parks: Insights from the Futian Mangrove Ecological Park, Shenzhen. Forests, 2024, 15(12): 2088. DOI:10.3390/f15122088
    6. Binaya Adhikari, Suresh C. Subedi, Shivish Bhandari, et al. Climate‐driven decline in the habitat of the endemic spiny babbler (Turdoides nipalensis). Ecosphere, 2023, 14(6) DOI:10.1002/ecs2.4584
    7. Jonathan O. Hernandez, Muhammad Naeem, Wajid Zaman. How Does Changing Environment Influence Plant Seed Movements as Populations of Dispersal Vectors Decline?. Plants, 2023, 12(7): 1462. DOI:10.3390/plants12071462
    8. Lu Feng, Xiaoming Ma, Alice C. Hughes, et al. Elevation range and contemporary climate determine the taxonomic, functional and phylogenetic diversity of forest mammals. Biodiversity and Conservation, 2023, 32(14): 4651. DOI:10.1007/s10531-023-02715-7
    9. Muhammad Nawaz Rajpar, Shahab Ali Khan, Allah Ditta, et al. Subtropical Broad-Leaved Urban Forests as the Foremost Dynamic and Complex Habitats for a Wide Range of Bird Species. Sustainability, 2021, 13(23): 13021. DOI:10.3390/su132313021
    10. O. Serebryakov, E. Turchaninova. INFLUENCE OF RECREATIONAL LOAD ON ORNITOFAUNA OF THE CITY OF VORONEZH. Materials of the All-Russian Youth Scientific and Practical Conference "Modern problems of the ecology of the animal and plant world", DOI:10.34220/MPEAPW2021_88-93

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(3)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (409) PDF downloads (8) Cited by(10)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return